The Guardian (USA)

The neuroscien­ce behind why your brain may need time to adjust to ‘un-social distancing’

- Kareem Clark

With Covid vaccines working and restrictio­ns lifting across the country, it’s finally time for those now vaccinated who have been hunkered down at home to ditch the sweatpants and re-emerge from their Netflix caves. But your brain may not be so eager to dive back into your former social life.

Social distancing measures proved essential for slowing viral spread worldwide – preventing upward of an estimated 500m cases. But, while necessary, 15 months away from each other has taken a toll on people’s mental health.

In a national survey last fall, 36% of adults in the US – including 61% of young adults – reported feeling “serious loneliness” during the pandemic. Statistics like these suggest people would be itching to hit the social scene.

But if the idea of making small talk at a crowded happy hour sounds terrifying to you, you’re not alone. Nearly half of Americans reported feeling uneasy about returning to in-person interactio­n regardless of vaccinatio­n status.

So how can people be so lonely yet so nervous about refilling their social calendars?

Well, the brain is remarkably adaptable. And while we can’t know exactly what our brains have gone through over the last year, neuroscien­tists like me have some insight into how social isolation and resocializ­ation affect the brain.

Social homeostasi­s – the need to socialize

Humans have an evolutiona­rily hardwired need to socialize – though it may not feel like it when deciding between a dinner invite and rewatching Schitt’s Creek.

From insects to primates, maintainin­g social networks is critical for survival in the animal kingdom. Social groups provide mating prospects, cooperativ­e hunting and protection from predators.

But social homeostasi­s – the right balance of social connection­s – must be met. Small social networks can’t deliver those benefits, while large ones increase competitio­n for resources and mates. Because of this, human brains developed specialize­d circuitry to gauge our relationsh­ips and make the correct adjustment­s – much like a social thermostat.

Social homeostasi­s involves many brain regions, and at the center is the mesocortic­olimbic circuit – or “reward system”. That same circuit motivates you to eat chocolate when you crave something sweet or swipe on Tinder when you crave … well, you get it.

And like those motivation­s, a recent study found that reducing social interactio­n causes social cravings – producing brain activity patterns similar to food deprivatio­n.

So if people hunger for social connection like they hunger for food, what happens to the brain when you starve socially?

Your brain on social isolation

Scientists can’t shove people into isolation and look inside their brains. Instead, researcher­s rely on lab animals to learn more about social brain wiring. Luckily, because social bonds are essential in the animal kingdom, these same brain circuits are found across species.

One prominent effect of social isolation is – you guessed it – increased anxiety and stress.

Many studies find that removing animals from their cage buddies increases anxiety-like behaviors and cortisol, the primary stress hormone. Human studies also support this, as people with small social circles have higher cortisol levels and other anxietyrel­ated symptoms similar to socially deprived lab animals.

Evolutiona­rily this effect makes sense – animals that lose group protection must become hyper-vigilant to fend for themselves. And it doesn’t just occur in the wild. One study found that self-described “lonely” people are more vigilant of social threats like rejection or exclusion.

Another important region for social homeostasi­s is the hippocampu­s – the brain’s learning and memory center. Successful social circles require you to learn social behaviors – such as selflessne­ss and cooperatio­n – and recognize friends from foes. But your brain stores tremendous amounts of informatio­n and must remove unimportan­t connection­s. So, like most of your high school Spanish – if you don’t use it, you lose it.

Several animal studies show that even temporary adulthood isolation impairs both social memory – like recognizin­g a familiar face – and working memory – like recalling a recipe while cooking.

And isolated humans may be just as forgetful. Antarctic explorers had shrunken hippocampi after just 14 months of social isolation. Similarly, adults with small social circles are more likely to develop memory loss and cognitive decline later in life.

So, human beings might not be roaming the wild any more, but social homeostasi­s is still critical to survival. Luckily, as adaptable as the brain is to isolation, the same may be true with resocializ­ation.

Your brain on social reconnecti­on Though only a few studies have explored the reversibil­ity of the anxiety and stress associated with isolation, they suggest that resocializ­ation repairs these effects.

One study, for example, found that formerly isolated marmosets first had higher stress and cortisol levels when resocializ­ed but then quickly recovered. Adorably, the once-isolated animals even spent more time grooming their new buddies.

Social memory and cognitive function also seem to be highly adaptable.

Mouse and rat studies report that while animals cannot recognize a familiar friend immediatel­y after short-term isolation, they quickly regain their memory after resocializ­ing.

And there may be hope for people emerging from socially distanced lockdown as well. A recent Scottish study conducted during the Covid pandemic found that residents had some cognitive decline during the harshest lockdown weeks but quickly recovered once restrictio­ns eased.

Unfortunat­ely, studies like these are still sparse. And while animal research is informativ­e, it probably represents extreme scenarios since people weren’t in total isolation over the last year. Unlike mice stuck in cages, many in the US had virtual game nights and Zoom birthday parties (lucky us).

So power through the nervous elevator chats and pesky brain fog, because “un-social distancing” should reset your social homeostasi­s very soon.

Kareem Clark is a postdoctor­al associate in neuroscien­ce at Virginia Tech

This article is republishe­d from the

For her first few years of life, my daughter Ella likely thought the television played a single piece of content: the 1993 version of George Balanchine’s Nutcracker, starring as the title role one Macaulay Culkin, who spends the majority of the ballet running around stage and flourishin­g his arm as the corps of the New York City Ballet actually dances. My mother had mentioned the story to Ella one day and I’d found this free version, by chance, on YouTube. It was charming, and on the scale of Really Bad Things You Can Expose Your Child To, seemed pretty anodyne. As a millennial parent, I was highly attuned to this scale, spending my days wading through a morass of screaming headlines arguing that even a few minutes of screen time might set my child down the asocial, Vitamin D-deprived path of playing Fortnite 22 hours a day and subsisting on Soylent.

But as Ella got older, and started to suspect this magic screen might hold other treasures, I decided to get a handle on how to approach the television. What I learned helped me form a cornerston­e of our household’s tech philosophy for life with preschool children.

Firstly, today’s children’s programmin­g is wildly more fast-paced and frenetic than programmin­g of yore. Watch a few minutes of The Powerpuff Girls, which cuts every few seconds and sprouts neon colors that might render your TV visible from outer space, and it feels like snorting four tablespoon­s of espresso. Compare this to older children’s programs like Mister Rogers, shot by a single camera and featuring a man who speaks at half the speed he puts on his cardigan, and you’ll immediatel­y see the difference. The rapid cuts common in newer children’s shows cue the brain to perk up and refocus attention; scientists call this an “orienting response.” The more cuts in a given minute, the worse it is for your child.

“By design, television programs exploit our orienting response,” write Drs Dimitri Christakis and Frederick Zimmerman in The Elephant in the Living Room, which explores the effect of television on children. With few exceptions, there are more quick cuts now than there were before. For a glimpse at the upshot, take this horrifying finding from one of Christakis’ studies: for every hour of daily television that a child aged 0-3 watches, the child’s risk of developing attention problems consistent with ADHD increases by 9%.

Secondly, one of the activities most important for developing brains is the kind known in academic and medical fields as “serve and return interactio­ns” – and the presence of screens dramatical­ly reduces them. The idea is that the more communicat­ion a child experience­s and the more she or he is exposed to language, the more resilient and successful and social she or he will go on to be. Television is slightly better for children if you, the parent, “participat­e” in the experience of watching and engage them in conversati­on about what they’re watching. But to do so you have to be able to see the screen. Which led me to one of my biggest takeaways: don’t hand your child a personal tablet if you can help it. Watch on as big a screen as you’ve got. Why?

“Kids create a walled-off space,” Dr Jenny Radesky told me about tablet usage in the under-four-foot-tall set. She is one of the lead authors of the 2016 AAP digital media guidelines for young children, and runs a lab at the University of Michigan. In one study she conducted to determine how tablets affect parent-child interactio­ns, she watched as parents were forced to lie melodramat­ically over the back pillows of couches, neck craned at unnatural angles, as their offspring balled up and edged them out. The smaller the screen, the more tiny elbows come out.

(A separate issue altogether is the idea of handing your child a tablet or smartphone in the hopes that they might learn something from an app. Research shows that children under the age of five have an exceedingl­y challengin­g time learning from a 2D screen and translatin­g that into the 3D world without help, so unless you’re right next to your kid, playing along, you should probably give up on the idea that it’s enriching.)

Of course, no one is expecting that parents are going to stop Frozen every few minutes to quiz Junior on the benefits and detriments of being able to turn one’s world into ice. You’re likely putting on the cartoon so you don’t have to engage at all, and there are, at my conservati­ve and highly unscientif­ic estimate, 14 million other op-eds to be written about why American parents need to resort to screen time as they navigate a world without the social support they deserve. But at least if you can see the screen, you might be able to use it as a jumping off point for interactio­n.

“I know it’s a ludicrous request these days, because [TV is] being used as a babysitter, but I’m constantly asking parents, ‘Please watch with your child,’” Rosemarie Truglio, a legend in the children’s programmin­g space, told me. She’s the senior vice president of curriculum and content at Sesame Workshop, meaning she’s in charge of making sure that one of the most beloved and respected preschool shows is teaching its viewers the right stuff.

“If you do, you could extend the learning,” she said. “Talk about, or act out, the story” after watching. “That’swhen they learn. Use it as a springboar­d.”

So, what’s our family’s television philosophy now?

Minimize screen time. Opt for boredom, and ride out any impending tantrums in the hopes that building a Magna-Tile castle, or getting lost in a make-believe world, may engross our daughters for a bit. When we just don’t have the wherewitha­l, we put on something nice, slow, and calm. And we put it on a big screen, to increase the chance we can enjoy it together.

During the pandemic, did Ella, now five years old, watch Oklahoma! so many times I’d often come across her innocently singing “I’m just a girl who can’t say no” in the bathtub? Yes. Did I flagellate myself over it? Nope. I just struck up a conversati­on about Ado Annie, and regional accents, and how she orta brush her teeth before bed.

Sophie Brickman is a contributo­r to the New Yorker, the New York Times and other publicatio­ns, and the author of Baby, Unplugged: One Mother’s Search for Balance, Reason, and Sanity in the Digital Age (forthcomin­g from HarperOne)

 ?? Photograph: Chameleons Eye/Rex/Shuttersto­ck ?? ‘While we can’t know exactly what our brains have gone through over the last year, neuroscien­tists like me have some insight into how social isolation and resocializ­ation affect the brain.’
Photograph: Chameleons Eye/Rex/Shuttersto­ck ‘While we can’t know exactly what our brains have gone through over the last year, neuroscien­tists like me have some insight into how social isolation and resocializ­ation affect the brain.’
 ?? Photograph: ryasick/Getty Images ?? ‘Watch a few minutes of The Powerpuff Girls, which cuts every few seconds and sprouts colors that might render your TV visible from outer space, and it feels like snorting four tablespoon­s of espresso.’
Photograph: ryasick/Getty Images ‘Watch a few minutes of The Powerpuff Girls, which cuts every few seconds and sprouts colors that might render your TV visible from outer space, and it feels like snorting four tablespoon­s of espresso.’

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States