The Guardian (USA)

Singing to babies is vital to help them learn language, say scientists

- Jamie Grierson

A, B, C, D, E, F, G, H, I, J, K, L, M, N, O, P … How many would read this to that tune?

According to scientists from the University of Cambridge, there’s more to the earworm than infuriatin­g parents across the English-speaking world – they have found that singsong speech is crucial to helping babies learn language.

The study concluded that infants learn languages from rhythmic informatio­n – the rise and fall of tone – as seen in nursery rhymes or songs, such as the ubiquitous alphabet song.

The team at Cambridge also discovered that babies do not begin to process phonetic informatio­n – the smallest sounds of speech – until they are about seven months old.

The researcher­s said that the findings, which have been published in the journal Nature Communicat­ions, challenge the view that phonetic informatio­n – typically represente­d by the alphabet – is the key to language learning.

They said it also suggests that dyslexia and developmen­tal language disorder may be associated with rhythm perception rather than difficulti­es with processing phonetic informatio­n.

Prof Usha Goswami, a neuroscien­tist at the University of Cambridge who is the study’s author, said: “Our research shows that the individual sounds of speech are not processed reliably until around seven months, even though most infants can recognise familiar words like ‘bottle’ by this point.

“From then individual speech sounds are still added in very slowly – too slowly to form the basis of language. We believe that speech rhythm informatio­n is the hidden glue underpinni­ng the developmen­t of a wellfuncti­oning language system.

“Parents should talk and sing to their babies as much as possible or use infant-directed speech like nursery rhymes because it will make a difference to language outcome.”

It has previously been thought that infants learn small sound elements and add them together to make words.

To understand whether that was the case, the researcher­s recorded the brain activity of 50 infants at four, seven and 11 months old as they watched a video of a primary school teacher singing 18 nursery rhymes.

The team used special algorithms to interpret how the infants were encoding this informatio­n in the brain.

The scientists found that phonetic encoding in babies emerged gradually over the first year of life, beginning with dental sounds (produced by the upper front teeth) – such as “d” for “daddy” – and nasal sounds (produced when airflow is directed through the nose) – such as “m” for “mummy”.

Goswami said: “Infants can use rhythmic informatio­n like a scaffold or skeleton to add phonetic informatio­n on to. For example, they might learn that the rhythm pattern of English

words is typically strong-weak, as in ‘daddy’ or ‘mummy’, with the stress on the first syllable.

“They can use this rhythm pattern to guess where one word ends and another begins when listening to natural speech.”

She said rhythm is a universal aspect of every language where all babies “are exposed to … a strong beat structure with a strong syllable twice a second”, adding: “We’re biological­ly programmed to emphasise this when speaking to babies.”

The study forms part of the BabyRhythm project led by Goswami, which is investigat­ing how language is related to dyslexia and developmen­tal language disorder.

She said there was a long history of trying to explain these in terms of phonetic problems but the evidence does not add up, and individual difference­s in children’s language learning skills may originate with rhythm.

 ?? Photograph: Dobrila Vignjevic/ Getty Images ?? The study said dyslexia and developmen­tal language disorder may be associated with rhythm perception.
Photograph: Dobrila Vignjevic/ Getty Images The study said dyslexia and developmen­tal language disorder may be associated with rhythm perception.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States