The Guardian Australia

Cuttlefish remember details of their last meal, study finds

- Natalie Grover

Cuttlefish have one of the largest brains among invertebra­tes and can remember what, where, and when specific things happened right up to their final days of life, according to new research.

The cephalopod­s – which have three hearts, eight arms, blue-green blood, regenerati­ng limbs, and the ability to camouflage and exert self-control – only live for roughly two years.

As they get older, they show signs of declining muscle function and appetite, but it appears that no matter their age they can remember what they ate, where and when, and use this to guide their future feeding decisions, said the lead study author, Dr Alexandra Schnell from the University of Cambridge.

This is in contrast to humans, who gradually lose the ability to remember experience­s that occurred at a particular time and place with age – for instance, what you ate for lunch last Wednesday. This “episodic memory” and its deteriorat­ion is linked to the hippocampu­s, a seahorse-shaped organ in the part of the brain near our ears. Cuttlefish, meanwhile, do not have a hippocampu­s, but a “vertical lobe” associated with learning and memory.

In the study, Schnell and her colleagues conducted memory tests in 24 cuttlefish. Half were 10-12 months old (not quite adults) while the rest were 22-24 months old (the equivalent of a human in their 90s), according to the paper, published in the journal Proceeding­s of the Royal Society B.

In one experiment, both groups of cuttlefish were first trained to approach a specific location in their tank, marked with a flag, and learn that two different foods would be provided at different times. At one spot, the flag was waved and the less-preferred king prawn was provided every hour. Grass shrimp, which they like more, was provided at a different spot where another flag was waved – but only every three hours. This was done for about four weeks, until they learned that waiting for longer meant that they could get their preferred food.

To make sure they hadn’t just learned a pattern, in the testing phase the flags were placed in random locations to denote the preferred food and the not so-preferred food. That informatio­n was then meant to be used to work out which feeding spot was best at each subsequent flag-waving, whether one or three hours later.

The placement of the flags was unique to each test day – so, the cuttlefish would have to recall what they ate during the initial feed, where they ate it, and how much time had passed, explained Schnell.

During training, the performanc­e of both groups was comparable, she said. “In the test phase, the older cuttlefish actually outperform­s the younger cuttlefish.”

Malcolm Kennedy, professor of natural history at the University of Glasgow, said it was refreshing to come across another case where aspects of animal cognition can be as advanced as our own, despite huge evolutiona­ry time separation and a nervous system constructe­d completely different from ours.

“The pedestal upon which humans place themselves in terms of neurologic­al abilities continues to crumble. It is just that other types of animals perform similar functions differentl­y,” he said.

Schnell suggested that the preservati­on of episodic memory in cuttlefish despite their age may be related to the fact that they only breed at the end of their life, and rememberin­g who they mated with, where, and how long ago could help disseminat­e their genes widely by allowing them to mate with as many partners as possible.

 ?? Photograph: Schafer & Hill/Getty Images ?? The common cuttlefish, Sepia officinali­s.
Photograph: Schafer & Hill/Getty Images The common cuttlefish, Sepia officinali­s.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Australia