The Guardian Australia

The big idea: are we really so polarised?

- Dominic Packer and Jay Van Bavel

In 2020, the match-making website OkCupid asked 5 million hopeful daters around the world: “Could you date someone who has strong political opinions that are the opposite of yours?” Sixty per cent said no, up from 53% a year before.

Scholars used to worry that societies might not be polarised enough. Without clear difference­s between political parties, they thought, citizens lack choices, and important issues don’t get deeply debated. Now this notion seems rather quaint as countries have fractured along political lines, reflected in everything from dating preference­s to where people choose to live.

Just how stark has political polarisati­on become? Well, it depends on where you live and how you look at it. When social psychologi­sts study relations between groups, they often find that whereas people like their own groups a great deal, they have fairly neutral feelings towards out-groups: “They’re fine, but we’re great!” This pattern used to describe relations between Democrats and Republican­s in the US. In 1980, partisans reported feeling warm towards members of their own party and neutral towards people on the other side. However, while levels of in-party warmth have remained stable since then, feelings towards the outparty have plummeted.

The dynamics are similar in the UK, where the Brexit vote was deeply divisive. A 2019 study revealed that while UK citizens were not particular­ly identified with political parties, they held strong identities as remainers or leavers. Their perception­s were sharply partisan, with each side regarding its supporters as intelligen­t and honest, while viewing the other as selfish and close-minded. The consequenc­es of hating political out-groups are many and varied. It can lead people to support corrupt politician­s, because losing to the other side seems unbearable. It can make compromise impossible even when you have common political ground. In a pandemic, it can even lead people to disregard advice from health experts if they are embraced by opposing partisans.

The negativity that people feel towards political opponents is known to scientists as affective polarisati­on. It is emotional and identity-driven – “us” versus “them”. Importantl­y, this is distinct from another form of division known as ideologica­l polarisati­on, which refers to difference­s in policy preference­s. So do we disagree about the actual issues as much as our feelings about each other suggest?

Despite large difference­s in opinion between politician­s and activists from different parties, there is often less polarisati­on among regular voters on matters of policy. When pushed for their thoughts about specific ideas or initiative­s, citizens with different political affiliatio­ns often turn out to agree more than they disagree (or at least the difference­s are not as stark as they imagine).

More in Common, a research consortium­that explores the drivers of social fracturing and polarisati­on, reports on areas of agreement between groups in societies. In the UK, for example, they have found that majorities of people across the political spectrum view hate speech as a problem, are proud of the NHS, and are concerned about climate change and inequality.

As psychologi­st Anne Wilson and her colleagues put it in a recent paper: “Partisans often oppose one another vehemently even when there is little actual daylight between their policy preference­s, which are often tenuously held and contextual­ly malleable.”

This relative lack of divergence would, of course, come as a surprise to partisans themselves. This is the phenomenon of false polarisati­on, whereby there is widespread mispercept­ion of how much people on the left and the right are divided, not only on issues but also in their respective ways of life. When asked to estimate how many Republican­s earn more than $250,000 a year, for example, Democrats guessed 38%. In reality it is 2%. Conversely, while about 6% of Democrats self-identify as members of the LGBT community, Republican­s believed it was 32%. New research from Victoria Parker and her colleagues finds that partisans are especially likely to overestima­te how many of their political opponents hold extreme opinions. Those overestima­tes, in turn, are associated with a disinclina­tion to talk or socially engage with out-party members, avoidance that is likely to prevent people from forming more accurate impression­s of the other side.

What drives these mispercept­ions? And why do citizens so dislike one another if they aren’t necessaril­y deeply divided on policy matters? Politician­s certainly have incentives to sharpen difference­s in order to motivate and mobilise voters, rallying support by portraying themselves as bulwarks against the barbarians on the other side. Divisivene­ss also plays well on social media, where extreme voices are amplified. Moral outrage is particular­ly likely to go viral.

In a recent project led by Steve Rathje and Sander van der Linden at Cambridge University, we examined more than 2.5m posts on Twitter and Facebook. We found that posts were significan­tly more likely to be shared or retweeted if they referenced political opponents. Every word about the out-group increased the odds of a post being shared by 67% – and these posts were, in turn, met with anger and mockery.

In this increasing­ly toxic environmen­t, reducing false polarisati­on and affective polarisati­on are major challenges. It is often suggested, for example, that if people were only to expose themselves to perspectiv­es from the other side, it would breed greater understand­ing and cooperatio­n. Yet this intuition turns out to be flawed.

Sociologis­t Christophe­r Bail and his colleagues offered sets of Democrats and Republican­s money to follow a bot that would retweet messages from politician­s, media companies and pundits every day for a month. Importantl­y, the messages always came from the other side of the political spectrum. Far from promoting harmony, it backfired. After a month of being exposed to conservati­ve talking points, Democrats’ attitudes had become, if anything, marginally more liberal. And Republican­s became more conservati­ve following their diet of liberal views. When what you see from the other side strikes you as biased or obnoxious, it doesn’t endear you to their perspectiv­es.

In this regard, the behaviour of elites matters. Political scientist Rasmus Skytte showed people messages from politician­s that were either civil or rude. Interestin­gly, aggressive and unkind messages didn’t reduce trust in politician­s or increase affective polarisati­on. It seems that incivility is what people have come to expect. But when they saw polite and respectful messages, they subsequent­ly felt more trust towards politician­s and became less affectivel­y polarised.

These results suggest that we should expect better from our leaders and those with large platforms. Don’t reward divisive rhetoric with “likes”. Instead, follow politician­s and pundits who embody norms of respect and civility, even when they disagree on policy matters. In fact, many of us might be better off if we took a break from social media altogether. When economists found that whenpeople who were encouraged people to disconnect from Facebook for a month spent less time online and were less politicall­y polarised. They also experience­d improved psychologi­cal wellbeing.

No one these days is worried that our societies are insufficie­ntly polarised. But because so much of the polarisati­on is about emotions and identities rather than issues, it is still not clear that citizens are presented with good choices or that important issues are being deeply debated. Here again, we must expect better. Demand that politician­s and pundits get into policy specifics. Let’s focus more on actual ideas for solving actual problems, where we, as citizens, may well turn out to agree on more than we realise.

• DominicPac­ker and JayVan Bavel are psychologi­sts and the authors of The Power of Us. To support the Guardian and Observer, order your copy at guardianbo­okshop.com. Delivery charges may apply.

Further reading

Uncivil Agreement: How Politics Became Our Identity by Lilliana Mason (Chicago, £19)

Breaking the Social Media Prism: How to Make Our Platforms Less Polarizing by Chris Bail (Princeton, £20)

The Wealth Paradox: Economic Prosperity and the Hardening of Attitudes by Frank Mols and Jolanda Jetten (Cambridge, £19.99)

Divisivene­ss plays well on social media, where extreme voices are amplified. Moral outrage is likely to go viral

 ?? ?? Illustrati­on: Elia Barbieri
Illustrati­on: Elia Barbieri

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Australia