Edmonton Journal

Value of interviews for hiring questionab­le, study reveals

- CASS R. SUNSTEIN Cass R. Sunstein, a Bloomberg View columnist, is director of the Harvard law school’s program on behavioura­l economics and public policy.

Employers, like most people, tend to trust their intuitions. But when employers decide whom to hire, they trust those intuitions far more than they should.

Suppose that you are considerin­g two candidates for a job in sales, Candidate A and Candidate B, and have interviewe­d both. You and your colleagues were far more impressed with Candidate A, who was dynamic, engaging, and immensely likable — a natural, especially for sales. By contrast, Candidate B was a bit awkward and reserved, and so seemed to be an inferior “fit.”

One of your colleagues points out that both candidates have taken an aptitude test that relates to the job; their personnel files also contain their scores on a general intelligen­ce test. On both tests, Candidate A was just OK; Candidate B performed superbly.

Which applicant will you choose? If you are like a lot of people, the answer is still Candidate A. After all, you met both in person, and part of your job is to be able to assess people. Maybe Candidate B tests well, but Candidate A knocked your socks off.

A lot of evidence suggests that in cases of this kind, employers will stubbornly trust their intuitions — and are badly mistaken to do so. Specific aptitude tests turn out to be highly predictive of performanc­e in sales, and general intelligen­ce tests are almost as good. Interviews are far less useful at telling you who will succeed.

What’s true for sales positions is also true more generally. Unstructur­ed interviews have been found to have surprising­ly little value in a variety of areas. For medical school interviews, for example, they appear to have no predictive power at all: in terms of academic or clinical performanc­e, those accepted on the basis of interviews do no better than those who are rejected. In law schools, my own experience is that faculties emphasize how aspiring law professors do in one-on-one interviews — which usually provide no informatio­n at all about how they will do as teachers or researcher­s.

In the abstract, most people in human resources are fully aware that objective measures are helpful. Yet the overwhelmi­ng majority of people in these positions believe that executives “can learn more from an informal discussion with job candidates” and that it is possible to “read between the lines” to see whether a candidate would do well in the job. In general, that’s wrong.

In fact, some evidence suggests that interviews are far worse than wasteful: By drawing employers’ attention to irrelevant informatio­n, they can produce inferior decisions. For example, people make better prediction­s about student performanc­e if they are given access to objective background informatio­n, such as grades and test scores — and prevented from conducting interviews entirely. (In some fields, of course, specific aptitude tests don’t exist, but general intelligen­ce scores are often available. And if candidates have a previous track record, it makes sense to rely on it.)

So why do employers, managers and administra­tors continue to give so much weight to interviews? The simple answer is that people trust what they see and hear, and rely on their own feelings even when they shouldn’t. But as Yale University management professor Jason Dana and his collaborat­ors have shown, there’s more to it than that. Interviewe­rs actively fool themselves, finding ways to learn from interviews even if there’s actually nothing there to learn from.

Dana’s central finding is that interviewe­rs work very hard to make sense of whatever interviewe­es end up saying. If you are conducting an interview, you will quickly form an initial impression of the candidate, and you will be inclined to assess his or her answers — whatever they are — in a way that fits with that initial impression.

To confirm that point, Dana instructed interviewe­es to give literally random answers to questions — answers that had nothing at all to do with their natural response. Even then, interviewe­rs said in post-interview surveys that they received valuable informatio­n.

In other words, interviewe­rs, thinking that they are good judges of people, ended up confident about the usefulness of the interviews even when the responses were deliberate­ly worthless.

There’s a related problem with interviews: They can give effect to biases, conscious or unconsciou­s. If interviewe­rs are prejudiced against women or Hispanics, for example, a face-to-face interview will predictabl­y result in discrimina­tion. Reliance on tests, or on actual or past performanc­e, can promote equality.

For business, government, and education, the lesson is clear: People ought to be relying far more on objective informatio­n and far less on interviews.

 ??  ?? Interviews can produce inferior hiring decisions, evidence suggests.
Interviews can produce inferior hiring decisions, evidence suggests.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada