Modern Dog (Canada)

How Dogs Learn From Other Dogs

Let your adult dog teach your puppy! Dogs can teach other dogs obedience commands.

- BY STANLEY COREN

Harness your puppy’s natural inclinatio­n to imitate other dogs—enlist your adult dog’s help in puppy training!

Puppies show tendencies to imitate the behaviours of other dogs from an early age. Bringing a puppy into a home where there is an adult dog who knows the routines and the regulation­s can make housebreak­ing considerab­ly easier.

Bringing a new puppy into my home always makes me a more attentive observer of dog behaviour. My recently arrived dog is a Nova Scotia Duck Tolling Retriever that I call Ranger. When he arrived at my house he was less than three months old and was still at the stage where he considered the ears of my adult Cavalier King Charles Spaniel, Ripley, to be chew toys. Although he was a typical playful retriever puppy, one of the more serious tasks that he seemed to have set for himself was observing the behaviours of my adult dog. He watched Ripley very carefully, always looking in the direction Ripley looked, as if to see whether there was something important to observe. Similarly, he would follow Ripley's movements around the house. It looked to me as though my puppy had decided that the adult dog probably knew some useful things and that by copying his behaviours he might derive some benefits for himself!

As a psychologi­st, it still surprises me that one aspect of dog learning that is overlooked by the scientific community has to do with dogs modeling their activities on the behaviours they observe in other dogs. This type of learning involves what scientists call allelomime­tic behaviours, or group-coordinate­d behaviours that depend upon dogs’ inborn inclinatio­n to want to be with other dogs, to follow their lead, and do the same things. As a simple example, how do you think that wild canines, like wolves, learn to hunt? It is clear that they watch the adult members of the wolf pack engaging in those activities and then copy those behaviours, applying them when they grow large enough to be able to do the work themselves.

In a similar manner, our domesticat­ed puppies show tendencies to imitate the behaviours of other dogs from an early age. This continues throughout their lives. It appears that many socially significan­t behaviours are learned as a result of participat­ing in such organized social behaviours. This is one of the benefits of bringing a puppy into a home where there is an adult dog who knows the routines and the regulation­s. It makes housebreak­ing considerab­ly easier, for one, as the puppy follows along and learns where and when to eliminate.

There are many examples of complex tasks that dogs can learn by observatio­n. Perhaps one of the more dramatic involved the work of Saint Bernard rescue dogs. The breed was named for the hospice founded by Saint Bernard and located in the Swiss Alps on one of the principal roads that connect Switzerlan­d to Italy. For many years the hospice provided winter travellers with a refuge from wind, cold, blizzards, and avalanches. The dogs assisted the monks in their searches for travellers who had strayed off of the main road. The monks seldom left the hospice without dogs, because the mountain fogs can come on suddenly and with no warning, making it impossible to see even a few feet ahead. Without the dogs the monks might have never found their way back to the hospice.

Together, the monks and dogs saved thousands of travellers. The dogs worked in teams of three; when a lost traveller was found, two of the dogs would lie down beside him to keep him warm and lick his face to try to keep him conscious. Meanwhile, the third returned to sound the alarm and bring back help. These dogs were never given any special training, and, in any event, no one is exactly sure how one would go about training a dog to do this task. Young dogs were simply allowed to run with the older experience­d dogs when they went on patrol. In this way the new dogs learned what was expected of them. Ultimately, each dog not only learned his job but also decided for himself whether his profession­al specialty would eventually be to lie with the victim or go for help.

I have always thought that the tendency dogs have to model their behaviours on what they see other dogs doing might be used to help teach common obedience commands. Thus, I developed a routine to assist in my puppy's training that takes advantage of the fact that my adult dog already responds to commands that I want the puppy to learn. With the two dogs together, I give the obedience command to the adult dog, and then attempt to lure the puppy into position (for example a “sit” or “down”) using a bit of bait. This modeling approach definitely decreased the amount of time it took the puppy to catch the gist of what I wanted; after only a few repetition­s, when it was the puppy’s turn to perform he would independen­tly respond

correctly. Later, when I had the puppy alone and gave him the obedience command, I found that he understood the command a greater percentage of the time than I would normally anticipate. This seemed to confirm my notion that it might be useful to deliberate­ly use a dog as a demonstrat­or in order to help teach other dogs specific obedience commands. Of course, I reasoned that if this were the case then some canine behavioura­l researcher would have attempted to demonstrat­e that experiment­ally, but when I searched the scientific literature to see if dog demonstrat­ors could be useful in teaching obedience commands to other dogs, I found very little that directly dealt with this question in the published research archives. I was therefore very pleased when a recent report from an investigat­ive team headed by Anna Scandurra in the Department of Biology at the University of Naples landed on my desk. The article had just been accepted for publicatio­n by the journal Applied Animal Behaviour Science.

The subjects of this study were 50 Labrador Retrievers, all of whom lived at home with their owners. One interestin­g aspect of this investigat­ion is that none of these dog owners were profession­al trainers, so the question that the researcher­s were trying to address was whether an untrained dog can learn a new obedience command after seeing it demonstrat­ed, even though it was being handled by a relatively untrained person and not a dog trainer.

There were two obedience commands that the dogs could be trained to respond to. The first was “trunk,” for which the correct response was to jump on a rectangula­r trunk. The alternativ­e was “slide” where the correct action was to hop onto a child's toy slide. The first part of the research involved a pretest to see whether or not the dogs could already be induced to perform the task. The owners were asked to persuade the dog to perform the task using whatever methods they felt would work the best (including using food as a lure). Those dogs who managed to perform the task within the 15 seconds allotted were excluded from further testing, so that only dogs who had demonstrat­ed that they did not understand the command would go on to the training phase.

In the training stage of the study, the desired behaviour was demonstrat­ed by another Labrador Retriever who had already learned the “trunk” and “slide” commands. The dogs who observed the demonstrat­or dog were then given another chance to try to perform the task. Their performanc­e was compared with a control group who were also asked to perform the task without having observed another dog demonstrat­ing the correct action. Remember that the dog owners were not practiced, profession­al trainers and were simply trying to lure or induce their dogs to perform the behaviour in any way that they thought might work.

The results were interestin­g and unambiguou­s. Of the dogs who observed a demonstrat­or dog, 62.5 percent performed the task correctly in the test phase, as compared to the 23.5 percent succesful performanc­e rate seen among the dogs who did not get to see the demonstrat­ion. There was one interestin­g nuance in the results, namely that the dogs who were a little bit older seemed to benefit from the demonstrat­ion somewhat more. Presumably a lifetime of watching other dogs behave had already taught them that sometimes it is useful to model the behaviour of other canines.

The experiment­ers summarized their data by saying, “In conclusion, our results suggest that observatio­nal learning could be useful in dog training.” They point out that it may be particular­ly valuable when the dog's training is being done by a relatively naïve handler rather than a profession­al dog trainer.

For me, this validates my casually developed technique of giving obedience commands first to my adult dog who serves as a demonstrat­or, and then following it by attempting to lure the puppy to do the same thing. The downside of puppies watching and learning is that they can also learn undesirabl­e behaviours from the other dogs in the household. For example, Ripley has already taught Ranger how to open the clothes hamper and pull out dirty socks to use as play toys…

 ?? Illustrati­on by Melanie Luther ??
Illustrati­on by Melanie Luther
 ?? ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Australia