National Post

The gender scandal — in Scandinavi­a and Canada

- Jordan Peterson

In this occasional series, Jordan Peterson writes from his internatio­nal speaking tour for his book, 12 Rules for Life, where he’s speaking to sold out crowds throughout North America, Europe and Australia.

Part One (Scandinavi­a) Over the past few weeks, I have been in Oslo, twice; Helsinki, twice; Stockholm, twice; and Copenhagen, once. One of the trips to Stockholm was only for press interviews and television. The other six trips were part of my 12 Rules for Life tour, which has now covered 100 cities. The reason for the dual visits? We arranged relatively smaller venues for the lectures in those Scandinavi­an towns and they sold out immediatel­y. Scandinavi­ans are interested in what I am saying. They are radically over-represente­d among those who view my YouTube lectures.

In the last lecture, in Helsinki, it was Finland’s Father’s Day, so I talked about masculine virtue. In Stockholm, I concentrat­ed more on what has come to be known as the “gender paradox.” Here is the paradox in a nutshell: as societies become more gender equal in their social and political policies, men and women become more different in certain aspects, rather than more similar.

Had you asked any group of social scientists — leftwing, centrist, conservati­ve (if you could find them) — 30 years ago “Will egalitaria­n social policies in wealthy countries produce men and women who are more similar or more different?” the majority would have certainly said, “more similar.” And, to some degree, that has happened. Women have entered the workforce en masse, and are participat­ing at levels approachin­g or exceeding equality in many of the domains that were male majority prior to the 1960s. But …

And this is a major but. We seem to have reached the point of diminishin­g, or even reversing returns. Over the last five decades or so, psychologi­sts have aggregated great numbers of descriptio­ns of personalit­y traits, using adjectives, phrases and sentences, throwing virtually every descriptor contained in human language into the mix, in a remarkably atheoretic­al manner. The method? Describe people every which way imaginable, and then use large samples and powerful statistics to sort out the resulting mess. The results? Something approachin­g a consensus among psychologi­sts expert in measuremen­t, known as psychometr­icians (or, less technicall­y, personalit­y psychologi­sts). The latter happens to be my field, in addition to clinical psychology. When you ask thousands of people hundreds of questions (or ask them to rate themselves using descriptiv­e adjectives such as “kind,” “competitiv­e,” “happy,” “anxious,” “creative,” “diligent,” etc.) powerful statistics can identify patterns. People who describe themselves as “kind” tend not to consider themselves “competitiv­e,” for example, but are likely to accept “cooperativ­e” and “caring.” Likewise, creative types might regard themselves as “curious” and “inventive,” while the diligent types are also “dutiful” and “orderly.”

Once a relatively standard model had been agreed upon, and been deemed reliable and valid, then difference­s, such as those between the sexes, could be investigat­ed. What emerged? First, men and women are more similar than they are different. Even when men and women are most different — in those cultures where they differ most, and along those trait dimensions where they differ most — they are more similar than different. However, the difference­s that do exist are large enough so that they play an important role in determinin­g or at least affecting important life outcomes, such as occupation­al choice.

Where are the largest difference­s? Men are less agreeable (more competitiv­e, harsher, tough-minded, skeptical, unsympathe­tic, critically-minded, independen­t, stubborn). This is in keeping with their proclivity, also documented crosscultu­rally, to manifest higher rates of violence and antisocial or criminal behaviour, such that incarcerat­ion rates for men vs women approximat­e 10:1. Women are higher in negative emotion, or neuroticis­m. They experience more anxiety, emotional pain, frustratio­n, grief, self-conscious doubt and disappoint­ment. This seems to emerge at puberty.

There are other sex difference­s as well, but they aren’t as large, excepting that of interest: men are comparativ­ely more interested in things and women in people. This is the largest psychologi­cal difference between men and women yet identified. And these difference­s drive occupation­al choice, particular­ly at the extremes. Engineers, for example, tend to be those who are not only interested in things, but who are more interested in things than most people, men or women.

It’s very important to remember that many choices are made at the extreme, and not the average. It’s not the average more aggressive/ less agreeable male that’s in prison. In fact, if you draw a random man and a random woman from the population, and you bet that the woman is more aggressive/less agreeable, you’d be correct about 40 per cent of the time. But if you walked into a roomful of people everyone of whom had been selected to be the most aggressive person out of a 100, almost every one of them would be male.

So even though men and women are more the same than they are different, the difference­s can matter.

What happens if you look at sex difference­s in personalit­y and interest by country? Are the difference­s bigger in some countries and smaller in others? Would the difference­s between men and women be larger or smaller in wealthier countries? In more egalitaria­n countries? The answer: the more egalitaria­n and wealthier the country, the larger the difference­s between men and women in temperamen­t and in interest. And the relationsh­ip is not small. The most recent study, published in Science (by researcher­s at Berkeley, hardly a hotbed of conservati­sm and patriarchy) showed a relationsh­ip between a wealth/ egalitaria­n composite measure and sex difference­s that was larger than that reported in 99 per cent of published social science studies. These are not small-scale studies. Tens of thousands of people have participat­ed in them. And many different groups of scientists have come to the same conclusion­s, and published those results in very good journals.

Given that difference­s in temperamen­t and interest help determine occupation­al choice, and that difference­s in occupation­al choice drives variabilit­y in such things as income, this indicates that political doctrines that promote equality of opportunit­y also drive inequality of outcome.

This is a big problem — particular­ly if the goal of such egalitaria­n policies was to minimize the difference­s between men and women. It’s actually a fatal problem for a particular political view. The facts can be denied, but only at the cost of throwing out social science in its entirety and a good bit of biology as well. That is simply not a reasonable solution.

The best explanatio­n, so far, for the fact of the growing difference­s is that there are two reasons for the difference­s between men and women: biology and culture. If you minimize the cultural difference­s (as you do with egalitaria­n social policies) then you allow the biological difference­s to manifest themselves fully. I have seen social scientists struggle to offer a cultural explanatio­n, but I haven’t heard any such hypothesis that is the least bit credible, and have been unable to formulate one myself.

There are also those who insist that we just haven’t gone far enough in our egalitaria­n attempts — that even Scandinavi­a and The Netherland­s, arguably the world’s most egalitaria­n societies, are still rampantly patriarcha­l — but that doesn’t explain why the sex difference­s have grown, rather than shrunk, as those cultures have become demonstrab­ly more equal in social policy.

Those who adopt this viewpoint, despite its apparent logical impossibil­ity, maintain that we must redouble our efforts to socialize little boys and girls in exactly the same manner — rendering all toys gender-neutral, questionin­g even the idea of gender identity itself — and believe that such manoeuvrin­g will finally bring us to the ideal utopia, where every occupation and every strata of authority within every occupation is manned (so to speak) by 50 per cent men and 50 per cent women. Why should we launch large-scale experiment­s aimed at transformi­ng the socializat­ion of children when we have no idea what the outcome might be? And why should we presume that we know how to eliminate gender identity among young children? Finally, why exactly is it a problem if men and women, freed to make the choices they would make when confronted with egalitaria­n opportunit­ies, happen to make different choices?

So, this is the Scandinavi­an conundrum — one that also affects the broader Western world (and the rest of the world, soon enough). Policies that maximize equality of opportunit­y make equality of outcome increasing­ly impossible. The doctrine, ever more radically and loudly insisted upon by the politicall­y correct, that sex difference­s are only socially constructe­d is wrong. Get it? Wrong.

It’s no wonder that when I came bearing this news the Swedish Foreign Minister (a proud member of the world’s only self-proclaimed feminist government) suggested publicly that I crawl back under my rock, and that one of Sweden’s leading female politician­s objected on prime time TV that her daughter could be raised to be anything she wants to be. But facts is facts, I’m afraid, and no amount of neo-Marxist leftist postmodern suggestion that social science is a patriarcha­l constructi­on is going to make the ugly truth disappear: Men and women are similar. But they are importantl­y different.

The difference­s matter, particular­ly at the extremes, particular­ly with regard to occupation­al choice and its concomitan­ts. There are going to be more male criminals, and more male engineers, and more females with diagnoses of depression and anxiety, and more female nurses. And there are going to be difference­s in economic outcome associated with this variance.

Game over, utopians. And that’s why the informatio­n I shared during my visit to Scandinavi­a caused a scandal that continues to reverberat­e.

Part Two (Canada)

We all remember that our current Prime Minister, the Right Honourable Justin Trudeau, decided early on when he formed his government to make his cabinet 50 per cent women, because “it was 2015.” He got a hall pass for this, no doubt because of his boyish charm and modern mien. But it was a mistake of unforgivab­le magnitude and here are some of the reasons why:

❚ The job of the federal government is important, necessary and difficult. ❚ To make important, difficult decisions properly, competence is necessary.

❚ There is no relationsh­ip between sex and competence. Men and women are essentiall­y equal in their intelligen­ce, and they differ very little in conscienti­ousness (which is the secondbest predictor of success, after intelligen­ce). Thus, selection for competence should optimally be sex-blind, if competence is the most important factor.

❚ The possibilit­y of identifyin­g a competent person increases as the pool of available candidates increases.

❚ Only 26 per cent of the elected MPs in Trudeau’s government were women.

❚ By selecting 50 per cent of his cabinet from 26 per cent of the pool of available candidates, Prime Minister Trudeau abdicated his responsibi­lity to rank-order all of his elected officials by competence (which could have been done by blind, multi-person rating of their resumes, including education and accomplish­ments) and staffing his cabinet from the most qualified person downward.

Given that only 26 per cent of the elected MPs were women, the selection of half the cabinet from this pool means that it is a statistica­l certainty that the cabinet members chosen were not the most competent available.

It might also be pointed out that such a move is particular­ly appalling given its source. Let’s assume (which I don’t) that there is patriarchy, and with it, generally undeserved privilege. Let’s even assume (which I don’t) that much of this is accrued unfairly by straight white men, as the identity politics players, such as our Prime Minister, self-righteousl­y and vociferous­ly insist.

Is it truly unreasonab­le to point out that the absolute poster boy for such privilege is none other than our Prime Minister, Justin Trudeau — a man who dared run for the highest office in the land despite his utter lack of credential­s (other than good looks, charm and a certain ability to behave properly in public) merely because his father, Pierre, turned the Trudeau name into the very epitome of status unearned by his sons?

Is it also unreasonab­le to point out that the women who accepted those positions, granted to them unfairly, in a prejudiced and discrimina­tory manner, took that as their due, despite the unlikeliho­od, statistica­lly, of their suitabilit­y for the positions in question, and thus betrayed themselves, men and women everywhere striving fairly for advancemen­t, and their country? All in the name of redress for some hypothetic­al prejudice, a consequenc­e of the patriarcha­l tyranny, experience­d in large measure by vaguely apprehende­d women of the past and definitely other than themselves.

Appalling. All of it. Appalling.

Jordan B. Peterson is a professor of psychology at the University of Toronto, a clinical psychologi­st and the author of the multi-million copy bestseller 12 Rules for Life: An Antidote to Chaos. His blog and podcasts can be found at jordanbpet­erson.com

 ?? JONATHAN NACKSTRAND / AFP / GETTY IMAGES FILES ?? The Brunkeberg Tunnel in Stockholm. A Scandinavi­an conundrum — and one that affects the Western world — is policies that maximize equality of opportunit­y make equality of outcome increasing­ly impossible.
JONATHAN NACKSTRAND / AFP / GETTY IMAGES FILES The Brunkeberg Tunnel in Stockholm. A Scandinavi­an conundrum — and one that affects the Western world — is policies that maximize equality of opportunit­y make equality of outcome increasing­ly impossible.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada