National Post (National Edition)

How is LeBron always one move ahead? Let’s ask the scientists

- Sally JenkinS

WASHINGTON • Much as his brute-strength shoulders and legs define LeBron James, it’s the stuff in his head that elevates him. If James has shown nothing else as the Cleveland Cavaliers struggle through the Eastern Conference finals, it’s the quality of his mind. What would a cellular analysis of his brain show? Maybe a flock of starlings.

There isn’t much reason to stay interested in an 0-2 series in which the Cavs have been borderline uncompetit­ive against the Boston Celtics in the fourth quarter, but it’s worth watching to the end if only to see whether James will pull off one of his memory tricks. His amazing recall has led to more than one comeback and is at least as much of a force as the form that produced 42 points, 12 assists and 10 rebounds in Game 2. Try a viewing experiment the next time James takes the court: analyze his head, and not his body. Watch him scan the game and store it upstairs.

Much has been made of James’ show-offy display of memory in his post-game analysis of Game 1. Replay it and notice not just the accuracy but the detail: In narrating six sequences in proper order, he noted the time on the shot clock, who took each shot and missed what, where the ball was inbounded from, and Jayson Tatum’s use of a Euro-step and right hand on a layup. When he was done, listeners broke into applause.

I ran James’ feat past some noted neuroscien­tists to see if it impressed them as much as it did the res tofus. “Fascinatin­g,” said Jocelyn Faubert, research chair in visual perception at the University of Montreal. “Quite beautiful really,” said Andre Fenton, professor of neuroscien­ce at New York Univer- sity. “It’s remarkable,” said Zach Ham brick, a cognition-performanc­e expert at Michigan State .“But not surprising .”

It’s not surprising because researcher­s are seeing an ever more articulate connection between cognitive science and human performanc­e.

“This is one of the bedrock findings in research on human expertise: that experts have superior memory for informatio­n within their domain,” Hambrick said.

Human performanc­e studies have shown that what seems to be “photograph­ic memory” is really extrapolat­ion based on habit-worn paths of knowledge, the vestiges and traces left in the brain by experience.

In a famous study of chess players by Adriaan de Groot in the 1960s, pieces were shown on a board for five seconds and then removed. The players were asked to recall what they had seen. Novices remembered only weakly. The more expert the player, the more pieces they could recall, and which piece was where.

“Grand masters could recall everything,” Florida State performanc­e researcher K. Anders Ericsson said.

But that’s not all. Masters of games don’t just build static memories, but show a remarkable ability to intuit. James’ anticipati­on is inseparabl­e from his memory.

In a study of elite soccer players cited by Ericsson, competitor­s were shown a game and the screen was halted at an unpredicta­ble point. Asked to recall the positions on the field, the best players not only remembered who was where, but also predicted where they would go next.

“They were able to assess where players are going, as opposed to where they are right now,” Ericsson said. “A momentary picture wouldn’t do that job.”

Hambrick cites a study of expert baseball and cricket players, which showed what happened when their view of the pitcher was partly obstructed: they became as confused as amateurs. This is because they are so used to having “advanced perceptual cues,” to predict where the ball will cross the plate.

Think about the processes involved as James scans the court while moving down the floor. The optic nerves absorb and transmit small peripheral details, then shift to a sudden zoom focus as he throws a glancing nolook bounce pass that hits Kevin Love in the hands in mid-stride. Then his attention broadens again stereoscop­ically to capture the whole floor. The cognitive flexibilit­y to go in and out of those states fluidly, is highly learned. And yet little short of magic.

“To manage all those systems, that is a form of intelligen­ce ,” Faubert said ,“and we shouldn’ t be afraid to say that.”

Most magical of all is what’s required to build those spatial maps in James’ head. In 2014, researcher­s John O’Keefe, May-Britt Moser and Edvard Moser won the Nobel Prize for explaining how the brain navigates. They answered a simple but profound set of questions: How do we perceive position, know where we are, find the way home? They discovered the brain’s “inner GPS” that makes it possible to orient and plan movement. O’Keefe found that a specific cell in the hippocampu­s throws off a signal to mark a specific place. The Mosers added to this by showing that neurons in the entorhinal cortex fire in fields with regularity. When they drew lines correspond­ing to the neuronal activity, here is what they saw: a grid. LeBron James has a geometric projection in his brain that acts a computatio­nal co-ordinate system. And so do you.

But wait. How does the hippocampu­s store distinct memories of similar events? How can James’ brain discrimina­te between multiple similar memories? Why don’t all crossover moves look as indistingu­ishable to him as spots in a parking garage? Fenton published a possible answer to this question in a paper this week in the journal Neuron. The “place” signalling by neurons is not so much a constant remapping, he suggested. It’s actually highly synchroniz­ed. Think of the neurons in James’ head as birds. Starlings. “Like a flock of starlings that takes on different formations while still maintainin­g cohesion as a flock,” Fenton said.

“He’s not recording, like a videotape,” Fenton says. “He’s not rebuilding. He doesn’ t rebuild a picture of what is going on, he watches it evolve continuous­ly and fluidly, there is a flock and it’s moving down the court and everybody has a place, all these birds form a structure, and the structure is important. We call it a flock. He call sit a play.”

We all have this remarkable combinatio­n of projection and flow in our heads, to varying degrees. You have it when you drive to work or do a job on deadline.

“It’s actually what you and I and all beings do,” Fenton said. “He’s not an enlightene­d being.”

Your brain has learned a series of models over your life and is constantly drawing computatio­ns. James’ just works better than yours on a basketball court because he has spent more time mapping that space.

“These people are masters of assessing the likelihood­s of things,” Fenton said. “If I have an amazingly good set of models and expectatio­ns — of my opponents, of my teammates and how the ball will move — it can look like I am totally omniscient.”

A FLOCK OF STARLINGS ... TAKES ON DIFFERENT FORMATIONS.

 ?? MADDIE MEYER / GETTY IMAGES ?? LeBron James’ head for the game is what really elevates him into a class of his own on the basketball court.
MADDIE MEYER / GETTY IMAGES LeBron James’ head for the game is what really elevates him into a class of his own on the basketball court.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada