Rotman Management Magazine

How to Increase Ethical Behaviour in Organizati­ons

Encouragin­g people to develop a vigilant mindset can go a long way towards reducing unethical behaviour in organizati­ons.

- By Ting Zhang, Pinar Fletcher, Francesca Gino and Max Bazerman

Developing a vigilant mindset and embracing four behavioura­l tools can go a long way in reducing unethical behaviour in organizati­ons.

FOLLOWING THE COLLAPSE of the world’s largest Ponzi scheme in 2008, clients of Bernard Madoff Investment Securities lost an estimated $65 billion in unrealized gains. In hindsight, investors detected major alarm bells that signaled Madoff’s returns were too good to be true: They were impossibly steady and high, at 11 per cent per year, never experienci­ng a down year, and were consistent­ly higher than those of the S&P 500 index, with surprising­ly little volatility.

Madoff employed a small, three-person auditing firm that had only one active principal accountant; the other principal was an 80-year-old retired accountant living in Florida. He was extremely secretive about his investment strategy and selective about who could invest in his funds. Despite these telltale signs of fraudulent behaviour, Madoff’s Ponzi scheme was not uncovered for more than a decade — a fact that surprised even Madoff himself. Although a few investors suspected something was up, many — including those with extensive knowledge of finance — were either completely blindsided or failed to act on their suspicions.

Behind this scandal were two groups of individual­s who contribute­d to the growth of the Ponzi scheme:

1. The perpetrato­rs, who knowingly committed unethical acts, including those at Madoff Investment Securities who created fraudulent records of non-existent transactio­ns; and

2. The investors, advisers and regulators who had the expertise and fiduciary responsibi­lity to detect fraudulent behaviour — but failed to do so.

This article will focus on the latter group, in an effort to understand the factors that can help people notice unethical behaviour — and act on their suspicions.

Ethics at Work: Key Findings to Date

Large-scale scandals including Madoff, Enron and Worldcom typically have a few ‘bad apples’ at the core, but are enabled by a larger group of individual­s who fail to notice and act — even when there are strong hints of wrongdoing. This has motivated

scholars to shift their focus from examining acts committed with the intention to do harm to investigat­ions of how individual­s who intend to do good are ultimately tempted to act unethicall­y.

This latter subset of unethical acts can be categorize­d as either intentiona­l (when individual­s are cognizant of their ethical violations) or unintentio­nal (when they are unaware that their actions cross ethical boundaries). In the domain of intentiona­l unethical behaviour, the study of ‘behavioura­l ethics’ — the systematic and predictabl­e ways in which individual­s fail to act ethically — demonstrat­es that although there are individual difference­s in people’s moral identity and ethical values, morality within an individual is also malleable. Mordechai Nisan’s moral balance model shows that people have a ‘moral bank account’, wherein good deeds ‘raise the balance’ and bad deeds lower it. Perception­s of a ‘surplus’ in one’s account might lead a person to cheat, whereas perception­s of a ‘low balance’ might lead one to act more ethically.

Elsewhere, psychologi­st James Rest’s model of unethical behaviour suggests that once an issue has been identified as having moral dimensions, individual­s engage in a moral judgment (i.e., they decide whether something is ethical or unethical), which triggers moral intention (they plan to act either ethically or unethicall­y) and moral behaviour (they act, either ethically or unethicall­y). Behavioura­l ethics research has also shown that peoples’ judgments, intentions and behaviour depend on the situationa­l and social forces in the environmen­t. For example, individual­s are more prone to cheat when they feel cognitivel­y depleted and can easily justify their behaviour; and because people often look to others to determine acceptable norms, people are also more likely to cheat when they see others cheating.

Alongside the research on intentiona­l acts of cheating is another body of research that studies how individual­s engage in

unintentio­nal acts of cheating. These behaviours fall under the broader category of bounded awareness — a state in which we systematic­ally fail to notice relevant informatio­n that falls outside of our attention at the time of making a decision.

Individual­s can also exhibit bounded ethicality, making unethical decisions that are outside of their own awareness and inconsiste­nt with their consciousl­y-held values. Both bounded awareness and bounded ethicality operate at an unconsciou­s level: Individual­s are not aware of how the biases that arise from their limited capacity to notice key informatio­n influence their judgments.

Examples of bounded ethicality include implicit prejudice and conflicts of interest. Even individual­s who espouse equality and diversity might discrimina­te based on gender or race without their awareness. Such implicit biases stem from stereotypi­cal associatio­ns that even individual­s who consciousl­y strive to be unbiased have difficulty overcoming. Furthermor­e, conflicts of interest can operate outside of an individual’s awareness. Auditors, for instance, may exhibit bounded ethicality when they fail to recognize how the promise of becoming a future employer for the audited firm precludes them from making impartial audits.

Because these biases operate at an implicit level, interventi­ons that would be aimed at addressing intentiona­l acts of cheating do not necessaril­y apply. Therefore, scholars are developing interventi­ons aimed at mitigating them. For example, recent research has demonstrat­ed that employers evaluating candidates separately exhibited a gender bias: They were more prone to hiring men for math-related tasks and women for verbal-oriented tasks, even when gender did not predict performanc­e on these tasks. In contrast, behavioura­l economist Iris Bohnet et al. found that evaluating male and female candidates jointly rather than separately eliminated reliance on gender stereotype­s in hiring decisions.

Whereas past research has aimed to reduce bounded ethicality that manifests as implicit biases, we recently set out to investigat­e factors that may help individual­s notice when options that appear attractive are in fact ‘too good to be true’.

Research on inattentio­nal blindness suggests that preparing peoples’ attention for a certain type of informatio­n or data significan­tly increases their chance of noticing that informatio­n. For example, when signs warning drivers about the presence of cyclists on the road are present, drivers become more likely to notice cyclists, which improves the safety of everyone on the road. We wondered, would preparing individual­s with a similar mindset of vigilance prior to making a decision lead them to attend to more informatio­n that challenges the viability of seemingly-attractive options?

To help us understand how creating a vigilant mindset affects the ability to notice elements of unethical behaviour and act on critical informatio­n, participan­ts in our study played the role of a financial advisor in an investment game. ‘Advisors’ were asked to select one fund to recommend to a hypothetic­al client from a pool of four funds. Unbeknowns­t to participan­ts, the fund with the most attractive risk-return profile — ‘Fortitude Investment­s’ — was based on Madoff’s feeder fund.

In addition to charts and graphs, individual­s could also read the fine print that contained important informatio­n about each fund. For the disguised Madoff feeder fund, the additional facts informed advisors that the fund was exclusive to investors with strong relations to the fund, opaque on disclosing its investment strategy, and maintained unconventi­onal auditing practices. Participan­ts who ignored this informatio­n and selected Fortitude Investment­s lost all of their clients’ money at the end of the hypothetic­al four-year investment period.

So, what percentage of individual­s selected Madoff’s feeder fund — despite having access to critical informatio­n about the fraudulent nature of the fund?

The answer depended on whether the ‘advisor’ selected the fund that appeared ‘most suspicious’ to them before or after they made their decision for the client. Those who made an investment decision for their client before contemplat­ing their suspicions selected Madoff’s feeder fund 68 per cent of the time; whereas those who made their decision after determinin­g which fund was the most suspicious selected the fund 51 per cent of the time.

In other words, instilling a vigilant mindset — by merely asking participan­ts to identify the fund that ‘seemed most suspicious’ to them prior to making a decision — reduced the propensity of selecting Madoff’s fund by 17 percentage points. The timing of instilling vigilance is critical to helping individual­s notice fraudulent behaviour and act on their suspicions: Once individual­s have already made a decision, they will be less likely to consider disconfirm­ing evidence and notice critical informatio­n.

Implicatio­ns for Management

Failure to notice unethical behaviour is a significan­t problem in modern organizati­ons — and in society, more broadly. Given the prevalence of both bounded awareness and bounded ethicality across organizati­onal contexts, our findings highlight the value of proactivel­y raising vigilance in helping individual­s consider reasons not to take a particular course of action.

Although increasing vigilance prior to making a decision decreased individual­s’ propensity to select Madoff’s fund, as indicated, more than half of the individual­s still recommende­d it. Clearly, vigilance is not enough. Following are some other proven tools that can complement a vigilant mindset.

1. ASK LOTS OF QUESTIONS Although this might seem obvious, many people fail to recognize that they can obtain more informatio­n than what is in front of them. Imagine that you are concerned that a supplier you are considerin­g for your company is using child labour. Directly asking, ‘Do you use child labour?’ is unlikely to be effective, but asking multiple related questions can help you determine whether the supplier is violating laws. Questions like, Can you break down the cost of materials and labour for me? How long does it take each worker to produce the product? Can you provide demographi­cs of your workers?, and Can you tell me how you manufactur­e these products? may indicate whether suspicion is warranted. You may realize, for instance, that it is impossible for the number of workers provided in their figures to produce all of products manufactur­ed, suggesting under-reporting of the number of employees.

2. ‘TRIANGULAT­E’ ON THE FOCAL ISSUE WITH MULTIPLE SOURCES Sometimes, investigat­ing one source is not enough. ‘Triangulat­ion’ is a powerful technique that consists of verificati­on from

People have a ‘moral bank account’ wherein good deeds ‘raise the balance’ and bad deeds lower it.

two or more sources. The idea is that one can be more confident with a result if different methods lead to the same result.

When asking multiple sources the same question, are the answers congruent with one another, or do they conflict? For example, consider the scenario that you are concerned a supplier is using child labour. If multiple sources reveal conflictin­g numbers about the number of employees working at the company, you have reason to be more suspicious. Inconsiste­ncy is often a sign that suspicion is warranted.

3. ATTEND TO ‘ARTFUL DODGES’ When people are asked questions that could reveal the unethical nature of a situation, they often give ‘non-answers’ in order to deflect attention away from the issue of concern. Such ‘dodges’ are signs that suspicions are warranted. For example, an individual who does not want others to know about the use of child labour may direct discussion to the quality of products or materials when asked about the low cost of labour. Research shows that individual­s tend to not notice when others are answering a slightly different question from the one that was asked. One way to detect artful dodges is to remember the original question and to think critically about whether the answer provided truly addresses the focal question.

4. LEAVE MORE TIME TO DECIDE After gathering all relevant informatio­n, leaving time to decide can prevent you from overlookin­g critical informatio­n. In a famous experiment, theologica­l seminary students were asked to give a lecture. Some of the priests were told that they needed to hurry in order to arrive at the building on time, whereas others were told they had plenty of time. On their way over to the lecture, all of the priests passed by a man slumped over, who appeared to be in need of medical attention. The priests who were not in a rush were more likely to help the man; while those in a rush were more likely to pass by without even noticing him. Time constraint­s proved to be an even bigger deterrent of helping behaviour than the content of the talk: Some of these seminary students were on their way to give a talk on being a Good Samaritan. As this study indicates, when people are in a rush, they a) do not have enough time to do any additional investigat­ive work, and b) fail to notice even when obvious informatio­n is in front of them.

In closing

By embracing a vigilant mindset as a ‘default state’, managers in any industry can develop a keen ability to notice unethical behaviour. In our view, this is a critical step towards reducing both bounded ethicality and bounded awareness in modern organizati­ons.

Most people don’t notice when someone answers a slightly different question from the one that was asked.

 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada