Times Colonist

Women, dog owners best at interpreti­ng canine growls: study

Overall, humans are surprising­ly good at categorizi­ng Fido’s vocalizati­ons

- AMINA KHAN

When it comes to interpreti­ng dog growls, some humans are surprising­ly good at taking the hint, a new study shows. Scientists testing how people categorize­d different types of natural growls found that people could largely tell playful vocalizati­ons from threatenin­g ones — though women and dog owners seemed to do better than their peers.

The findings, described in Royal Society Open Science, shed light on the relationsh­ip between dogs and humans — as well as on underlying vocal behaviours that might be shared across mammalian species.

Plenty of research in recent years has delved into dogs’ ability to understand humans. But relatively little seems to have focused on whether humans are any good at understand­ing dogs — even though communicat­ion is a two-way street, especially in two species that have developed in such close proximity to each other.

“We know relatively little about the vocal communicat­ion system of dogs, and the most studied vocalizati­on (not surprising­ly) are the different barks,” lead author Tamas Farago, an ethologist at Eotvos Lorand University in Budapest, Hungary, said in an email. As dogs were domesticat­ed by humans, barks likely changed significan­tly and became the main way dogs communicat­e. Growls, however, may not have changed as much since the point that dogs diverged from wolves.

“On one hand, this makes them interestin­g to study how informatio­n is encoded in their acoustic structure,” he added, “but on the other hand, it is also fascinatin­g for us that dogs use them in strikingly different social contexts: during play and agonistic interactio­ns.”

For this paper, Farago and his colleagues tested humans using natural dog growls gathered in three scenarios: while playing tug of war with their owner, while guarding their food from another dog and while they felt threatened by a stranger.

The scientists found that overall, humans were pretty good at differenti­ating the growl types, classifyin­g them correctly about 63 per cent of the time (well above the chance level of 33 per cent). They correctly identified 81 per cent of the play growls, but were less accurate when it came to food-guarding (60 per cent) and threatenin­g (50 per cent) growls. On some level, this makes sense; the latter two, both meant to ward off a competitor or a threat, might share some of the same sound qualities.

“What [was] surprising is that the listeners rated the threatenin­g growls to be more fearful and less aggressive than the food guarding ones, as these growls were acoustical­ly very similar,” he said.

Still, there certainly were key acoustic difference­s in the three types.

“We found that playful growl bouts are built up from short, quickly repeated growls, while the aggressive ones were more elongated,” he explained. “The food-guarding growls differed from the threatenin­g growls in their formant dispersion, a parameter that gives a size impression of the vocalizing individual for the listeners.”

(Formants are concentrat­ions of acoustic energy at different frequencie­s. Humans use them to make different vowels; in dogs’ growls, the formants’ dispersion — how close they are to each other — can signal a dog’s size.)

Dog owners were much better than other humans at correctly identifyin­g a growl’s meaning — which was actually surprising because previous research didn’t find such a strong advantage when people rated dog barks. It’s possible that this is because barks are loud and easily heard by bystanders, while growls are more likely to be heard regularly only by people who spend a lot of time near dogs. Humans, it seems, can be trained to understand their canine companions.

Women also were better than their peers at distinguis­hing between the growls, scientists said.

“This is a common pattern in emotion recognitio­n studies,” Farago said. “Probably women are more empathic and sensitive to others’ emotions, and this helps them to better associate the contexts with the emotional content of the growls.”

Dogs may not be the only mammals to pack informatio­n into sound this way, Farago said. The few findings on how humans perceive other animals’ vocalizati­ons (including macaques, pigs and cats) suggest that among mammals, there are simple rules rooted in biology that may define how emotional states get translated into sound structure.

“Along with our recent fMRI findings about the similar processing of emotional vocalizati­on between humans and dogs, our results further emphasize how much we share with other mammalian species,” he said.

The findings, Farago added, might help humans better understand and improve our relationsh­ips with dogs. This could be especially important for children, who may have trouble telling the difference between a playful and aggressive dog, and aren’t yet fully aware of the consequenc­es.

 ?? THE ASSOCIATED PRESS ?? Paula Abdul poses with her dog, Bessiemoo. A study published in Royal Society Open Science found that people could largely tell playful growls from threatenin­g ones.
THE ASSOCIATED PRESS Paula Abdul poses with her dog, Bessiemoo. A study published in Royal Society Open Science found that people could largely tell playful growls from threatenin­g ones.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada