Times Colonist

Birdsong gets sweeter over time, study finds

- TARPLEY HITT

MIAMI — On a dewy summer morning, Karla Rivera-Caceres, an ornitholog­y researcher at the University of Miami, crouched in her usual workspace — the tall grasses of Costa Rica’s woodland — and heard something unusual.

Rivera-Caceres studies birdsong, and that day she was listening to the canebrake wren, a brown bird whose bland appearance (it was once named the “plain wren”) belies an unusual and extremely complex call.

Canebrake wrens are songbirds, the subset of species whose calls develop beyond the standard tweet or chirp into fullfledge­d ballads — and within that group they are part of a somewhat exclusive club: duetting birds.

When two of these wrens communicat­e, they weave their songs into an elaborate, Sonny and Cher-style duet. They warble back and forth, literally finishing each other’s phrases, with such high co-ordination that, to an outsider, they sound like a single voice.

But as Rivera-Caceres sat listening that morning in 2011, she noticed something odd about this pair’s effort: Their duet was really bad.

The birds were sloppy. They chirped over each other. They sang the wrong responses and screwed up the timing. They were young, still inexperien­ced at singing, and it showed. As birdsong goes, their act was like a five-year-old belting opera. In the world of bird science, her observatio­n proved huge.

Namely: it demonstrat­ed that birdsong works even more like language than we think — that in order to achieve their Pavarottie­sque exchanges, wrens need to learn a specific set of social rules which are similar to what humans might call manners.

Rivera-Caceres’ discovery launched her on a years-long research project at the University of Miami. This year, her findings were published in the journal Royal Society Open Science, and are already altering the way scientists conceive of birdsong.

“People think humans are so unique,” Rivera-Caceres said, “But these birds are talking to each other, developing intimate codes. We’re not the only ones who communicat­e or hold complex thoughts.”

Humans have been fascinated by the music of birds for centuries. Expert birdwatche­rs learn to identify species they might not be able to see simply from their distinctiv­e calls. For scientists, calls and songs can tell different stories. According to a recent piece in the New York Times, researcher­s are increasing­ly finding that some birds are forced to alter songs to adapt to the surroundin­g din of humanity.

Comparison­s between birdsong and human language date back to Darwin, Rivera-Caceres said (“The sounds uttered by birds offer in several aspects the nearest analogy to language,” he wrote in The Descent of Man), and scientists have long studied how baby birds learn the tunes of their songs — just like infants learns new words. One recent study found that genes in songbirds might shed light on human speech disorders.

But Rivera-Caceres’ focus was more like an avian Emily Post — she was interested in how those birds learned to use those songs and how their interactio­ns reflected a set of shared social guidelines, much like the subtle norms that govern human conversati­on with friends, family and romantic partners.

If you aren’t a bird expert, the wrens’ song (loud, high-pitched and alternatin­g between notes sort of like a see-saw) might sound like something off the Rainforest setting of a whitenoise machine. But to the trained ear, their calls are highly specific and follow a strict set of rules.

“The rules are called ‘duet codes,’ ” said Rivera-Caceres. “Every time a male sings one song type, the female will answer with a certain other song type.” People do this too, RiveraCace­res said: when someone asks about weather, they expect an on topic answer, not a monologue on television or cooking.

The wrens’ tunes are also carefully timed to avoid pauses or interrupti­on. “If they overlap, the birds stop singing,” said RiveraCace­res. Aversion to pauses and interrupti­ons is also a convention of human conversati­on. That’s why, for example, people prefer not to speak over each other, or why delays between TV hosts and their correspond­ents can seem so awkward.

Human manners, as anyone with children will know, are learned with practice — often after many reminders not to interrupt. But when RiveraCace­res first proposed her project, most researcher­s believed wrens were born with instinctiv­e codes and rhythms, the way a chimp automatica­lly knows how to grasp or grunt.

Few had challenged this theory, in part because duetting wrens are so difficult to study, with their young hard to find in the wild. Even Rivera-Caceres’ adviser, professor William Searcy at the University of Miami, doubted she could break new ground.

“He said it would be very difficult. It’s hard to study a system that few-to-no people have studied,” Rivera-Caceres said. “But when I showed him these duets where the juveniles would do a terrible job at duetting with adults, he was excited.”

“I was excited because nobody had demonstrat­ed that juveniles were worse at duetting than adults,” Searcy said. “Nobody had shown that they improve over time.”

Rivera-Caceres’ discovery demonstrat­ed that wren-worldetiqu­ette was more like our own: that it was learned by trial and error.

It’s not clear if RiveraCace­res’ findings are necessaril­y applicable to other creatures, according to her longtime collaborat­or Christophe­r Templeton, an assistant professor at Pacific University in Oregon — but they do provide food for thought.

“It’s difficult to extrapolat­e to other species,” he said, “but I suspect that the patterns that we’re finding here are broadly applicable to other animals.”

Rivera-Caceres’ first breakthrou­gh opened the door for more findings.

The canebrake wrens use their elaborate duets to defend territory and attract mates. Before Rivera-Caceres’ research, a 1990s paper had argued that, unlike humans, wrens’ repertoire of songs was fixed from an early age. In order to mate, in other words, they had to find another bird who sang just like them.

Rivera-Caceres found otherwise. Mates frequently sing songs that were not used by their parents. In fact, it can be hard to find two pairs of birds, even within the same population, that follow the same exact set of rules.

She suspected that wrens developed their songbooks with their companions — just like friends form inside jokes, or couples create their own modes of communicat­ion.

To prove it, Rivera-Caceres captured several wrens in Costa Rica, and separated them from their mate, facilitati­ng a kind of avian divorce. It wasn’t as cruel as it sounds, she said. “They keep singing — like, where are you, where have you gone? But as soon as someone else arrives, they’re fine. They mate with the new bird.”

She set the birds up with new partners, matching them with wrens from other areas. Then, she watched the birds as they got to know each other; she heard them determine their duets.

In these new couples, Rivera-Caceres found that the wrens were like kids again: bad at timing, bad at responses. Some chirped when they should have trilled. Others interrupte­d and overlapped. They underwent — like many new couples — some problems with communicat­ion.

“Pairs that were together for less time made mistakes more often,” Rivera- Caceres said. “Instead of always answering with a certain song type, they would mess up and sing something else.”

But over time, that changed. After a while, the new pairs mastered their rhythms and responses. What’s more, they sang duet codes that they had not used with their previous mates. With their second partner, in other words, the wrens had developed a whole new way of relating.

“As they were together for a longer period of time, they figured it out,” she said.

According to Searcy, RiveraCace­res’ adviser, her work has paved the way for more insights.

“Karla has opened up a new area of investigat­ion in song developmen­t — and that is the developmen­t of interactio­n rules,” Searcy said. “I hope this becomes an active area of research in ornitholog­y. “

 ??  ?? University of Miami ornitholog­ist Karla Rivera-Caceres' work on the songs of tropical wrens has redefined understand­ing about bird communicat­ion.
University of Miami ornitholog­ist Karla Rivera-Caceres' work on the songs of tropical wrens has redefined understand­ing about bird communicat­ion.
 ?? CHRISTOPHE­R TEMPLETON ?? Two adult canebrake wrens share a branch as they perform the birds’ signature duet.
CHRISTOPHE­R TEMPLETON Two adult canebrake wrens share a branch as they perform the birds’ signature duet.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada