Toronto Star

The truth is out there. The problem is that we don’t believe it

Logic doesn’t play big role in our beliefs

- Edward Keenan

There are days, in this age of Trumpian “alternativ­e facts” and Trudeaupia­n selfies, of climate-change deniers and anti-vaxxers and all the varieties of “truthers,” when you begin to wonder if things like facts and evidence and logic play much of a role in our public debates anymore. There’s a tenor to the barrage of insults on social media that further raises the question.

And then there’s this: Less than half of supporters of U.S. President Donald Trump believe his son Donald Jr. attended a meeting with Russians about informatio­n that might be harmful to Hillary Clinton. A poll last month by Public Policy Polling found that 32 per cent of Trump supporters believe there was no such meeting, and a further 24 per cent say they’re not sure. This, the pollster reported, “is in keeping with the general attitude of Trump supporters,” of whom 72 per cent say the whole Russia story is “fake news.”

0The astonishin­g thing about that — astonishin­g to those of us outside the Trump-true-believer bubble, anyhow — is that Donald Trump Jr. is among our direct sources of informatio­n on that meeting and what it was about. In early July, the president’s son tweeted an email chain showing he had agreed to attend a meeting with someone who was said to be a “Russian government attorney” who had “very high level and sensitive informatio­n” that would be incriminat­ing to Clinton.

In other words, he has admitted it and provided evidence to back his admission. Yet his father’s supporters continue to believe the story is made up by the mainstream media and Trump opponents.

There are clues all around that make you suspect we live in an Age of Unreason. And then there is this — a smoking gun in a red hand that seems to close all room for doubt.

Way back at the dawn of modern western civilizati­on, Aristotle laid out the three modes of persuasion that are used to make arguments: ethos (the credibilit­y of the speaker), pathos (the emotions of the audience) and logos (logic or reason applied to the facts of the matter at hand).

Today, it is all too clear that the last of these — perhaps long thought to be the most valid and important — plays only a small role in how we discuss and decide things.

Ethos continues to play a prominent role. In online debates among the left, the credibilit­y of a speaker, hinged on their identity (race, gender, sexual orientatio­n, class), often seems to be a trump card.

For those on the right, the alignment of a news organizati­on (Breitbart versus the Failing New York Times, say) may be more persuasive than the reporting itself. Who is speaking, and how we regard them based on their identity and political alignment, seems to remain a big part of how we choose what to believe.

Pathos, the emotional argument, looms even larger, from the resentment­s that fuel political debates to the offences that fuel campus complaints to the hope and “sunny ways” feelings that have driven the success of candidates such as Barack Obama and Justin Trudeau and Jack Layton. Often enough, it certainly seems that for many people a feeling is an argument in itself. And what about Logos? What about reason? It isn’t that we’ve discarded it altogether. Just that it’s increasing­ly clear we use it pretty selectivel­y and mostly to confirm the conclusion­s we’ve drawn based on the emotions and identity affiliatio­ns we use to make up our minds.

There’s a significan­t and constantly growing body of research that shows that we use reason — we evaluate the facts and apply logic — primarily to convince ourselves and others that we are right.

You can go online and look up “confirmati­on bias” or “motivated reasoning” and see that studies show, again and again over a period of years, people seek out and believe informatio­n that aligns with what they already believe and to disbelieve or avoid any informatio­n that contradict­s those beliefs.

“Facts don’t change our minds” as the title of a New Yorker article from earlier this year put it — and when we encounter facts that contradict what we believe, we tend to stop trusting the person giving us the facts, rather than revise our longstandi­ng belief. Such an experience may, paradoxica­lly, reinforce our belief.

It’s easy to see this tendency in those we disagree with — of course I led off with an example about Trump supporters, who seem obviously wrong to me, and I receive dozens of letters a week from people who claim to see my bias and illogic equally clearly.

But there’s no reason to think this tendency to make up our minds first, seek evidence later and think ill of anyone who disagrees with us is limited to one group of people or one side of the political spectrum.

A study reported in an upcoming issue of the Journal of Experiment­al Social Psychology recently summarized in an article in Vox found that pretty much everyone — liberals and conservati­ves alike — will do a lot to avoid even finding out what their opponents think or why they think it.

In both the United States and Canada, they found people would forgo money in order to avoid hearing about the political thinking of those they disagree with. Asked to place the experience of hearing the political views of opponents on a desirabili­ty spectrum, those on the left and right each rated it almost as bad as having a tooth pulled.

“Largely, the partisans were unfamiliar with their viewpoints. So it’s not the case that people are avoiding learning about the other side because they’re already familiar,” the Vox piece says. “‘People on the left and right,’ the study concludes, ‘are motivated to avoid hearing from the other side for some of the same reasons: the anticipati­on of cognitive dissonance and the underminin­g of a fundamenta­l need for a shared reality with other people.’ ” This may not actually be new. If it is true, as it seems, that it is part of how our brains work to make up our minds first and seek evidence later and to avoid even hearing about why some people disagree with us, then it seems equally likely it may always have been the case. And yet for many of us, this tendency seems increasing­ly pronounced recently.

To me, it seems that some politician­s, especially conservati­ve politician­s, are particular­ly brazen in disregardi­ng evidence. Former Conservati­ve justice minister Rob Nicholson, who served under Stephen Harper, told Maclean’s magazine in 2009 and 2010 fairly straightfo­rwardly that when it came to getting tough on crime, statistics about crime levels and evidence about whether policies would be effective were secondary to his government and the “feeling” of voters was more important.

Rob Ford as mayor of Toronto seemed to take disregardi­ng evidence beyond gut feeling and openly despising expertise to an extreme, while Trump in the U.S. seems to have only further extended this longstandi­ng right-wing trend.

To others, it seems equally apparent in recent years that the appeals of both Obama and Trudeau were based on platitudes about hope and “sunny ways” — though both talked about evidence-based decision making and logic, their popular appeal was about a feeling of optimism their supporters wanted to share.

And beyond the ballot box, you can see the emotional arguments, and the shutting down of any but the most toxic debate, pretty much everywhere. The internet has broken up the informatio­n monopoly of the big pseudo-profession­alized media giants who pride themselves (maybe too much, but pride themselves still) as objectivit­y-seeking honest brokers. Today, anyone with a theory and a web connection can provide news and informatio­n, unfiltered by skeptical reportoria­l middlemen, directly to an audience looking for it.

Social media seems to be a steroid strengthen­ing the tendencies: on Facebook, friend selection and algorithms mean one can find a circle of news, informatio­n and discussion made up almost exclusivel­y of like-minded people sharing stuff you already believe to be true. On Twitter, the partisan lines of the short-form argumentat­ive bursts are drawn like trenches in warfare — much disagreeme­nt consisting merely of accusation­s of malice or bias or dishonesty lobbed back and forth, with people who share any controvers­ial opinion subjected to a perceived mobbing as hundreds or thousands of accounts rush to tweet insults at them. We may not be any more unreasonab­le than in the past, but if this feels like the Age of Unreason, perhaps it is because the science demonstrat­ing it has piled up at the same time that our technology and politics make it more transparen­t to us than ever before.

The problem is obvious enough: our society and political system are based, in large measure, on the premise that disagreeme­nts can be resolved and compromise­s can be reached when reasonable people attempt to persuade each other. If we’re all unreasonab­le, and are avoiding each other’s attempts to persuade, what can we do?

It’s not an abstract question, either. Policy questions, life and death decisions, hinge all the time on democratic resolution of disagreeme­nt.

And in a democracy, you cannot actually defeat your opponents — at least not all of them — instead, you must persuade them. Given what we know in the Age of Unreason, how can we do that?

The science about this, as I understand it, offers few satisfying conclusion­s. Very few of the papers on motivated reasoning seem to suggest any reliable ways of getting around it.

For each of us, assuming we want to be reasonable, perhaps being aware of all of this is a first step — we can attempt to be aware of our biases and perhaps seek out informatio­n that might change our minds. As unpleasant as we find it, perhaps anyone serious about public discussion needs to seek out arguments we disagree with in order to at least attempt to understand them. If not to be convinced ourselves, at least to be able to know our grounds for disagreeme­nt.

And perhaps knowing that identity and emotion play such a large role, we need to seek to address them in any arguments we make if we’re hoping to persuade people. People are not going to change their sense of themselves, and their emotions are real to them whether they seem justified to other people or not. So it would seem — logically, to me — that attempts to persuade people would need to leave room for their sense of selves and the perceived validity of their own emotional reactions.

And then there’s that thing the researcher­s cited in Vox said about why we avoid different opinions in the first place: “the fundamenta­l need for a shared reality with other people.” Perhaps acknowledg­ing or emphasizin­g shared humanity is part of how we can come to agree or at least disagree reasonably. Some research exists showing the possibilit­y that short, personal door-to-door conversati­ons by LGBTQ canvassers were effective in reducing the transphobi­a of those they conversed with.

(There’s a strange story behind this reported by Wired magazine: a 2014 study claimed to show exactly this effect in a gay marriage canvass. It was quickly shown to be fraudulent, and the research was retracted. But then in 2016, the people who debunked the original paper conducted the experiment for real trying to address transphobi­a, and got the same result as was originally reported.)

It does seem to me like the realizatio­n of shared humanity — and friendship, family ties, shared identity — should be effective in creating an emotional connection that might open people’s minds to more productive discussion. To me, it seems reasonable. But perhaps I am motivated to think so because I want to believe there’s a way to have a productive conversati­on, and I’m looking for evidence to support me in that belief.

If that’s the case, at least I know I’m not alone in doing so. Today in Insight Stories that flipped the script, IN1 Why we don’t stop believing, IN4 For whom the web trolls, IN4 On agreeing to disagree, IN6 The power of emotional ads, IN7

 ??  ??
 ??  ??
 ?? EDUARDO MUNOZ ALVAREZ/AFP/GETTY IMAGES ?? Trump supporters decried reports about Donald Trump Jr. meeting with Russians. Never mind that he admitted it.
EDUARDO MUNOZ ALVAREZ/AFP/GETTY IMAGES Trump supporters decried reports about Donald Trump Jr. meeting with Russians. Never mind that he admitted it.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada