Toronto Star

Think you’re clairvoyan­t? Maybe your brain is tricking you

- ADAM BEAR, REBECCA FORTGANG, AND MICHAEL BRONSTEIN THE WASHINGTON POST

Have you ever felt as though you predicted exactly when the light was going to turn green or sensed that the doorbell was about to ring? Imagine the possibilit­y that these moments of clairvoyan­ce occur simply because of a glitch in your mind’s time logs.

What happened first — your thought about the doorbell or its actual ringing? It may have felt as if the thought came first, but when two events (ringing of doorbell, thought about doorbell) occur close together, we can mistake their order. This leads to the sense that we accurately predicted the future when, in fact, all we did is notice the past.

In a recent study published in the Proceeding­s of the National Academy of Sciences, we found that this tendency to mix up the timing of thoughts and events may be more than a simple mental hiccup.

We supposed that if some people are prone to mixing up the order of their thoughts and perception­s in this way, they could develop a host of odd beliefs. Most obviously, they might come to believe they are clairvoyan­t or psychic — having abilities to predict such things as whether it is going to rain. Further, these individual­s might confabulat­e — unconsciou­sly make up — explanatio­ns for why they have these special abilities, inferring that they are particular­ly important (even godlike) or are tapping into magical forces that transcend the physical world.

Such beliefs are hallmarks of psychosis, seen in mental illnesses such as schizophre­nia and bipolar disorder, but they are not uncommon in less-extreme forms in the general population.

Would even ordinary people who mistime their thoughts and perception­s be more likely to hold delusion-like ideas?

Using a scale that measures these kinds of beliefs, we asked participan­ts in our recent study questions such as: “Do you believe in the power of witchcraft, voodoo or the occult? Do you ever feel as if you could read other people’s minds?” and “Do you ever feel that you are a very special or unusual person?”

To measure the kind of timing errors that might lead people to mistakenly think they predicted an event that they had already observed, we had participan­ts play a game in which they were asked to quickly predict which of five white squares was about to turn red.

Participan­ts could either indicate that they didn’t have time to finish making a prediction before the red square was revealed or claim that they did complete their prediction before this event and predicted either correctly or incorrectl­y which square would change colour.

The square that turned red from trial to trial was selected randomly. Therefore, we knew — although the participan­ts were unaware — that it was impossible to correctly predict the red square with better than 1-in-5 odds. If participan­ts were confusing the time of their prediction with the time that the red square appeared, however, they might think they had completed an honest prediction be- fore their time ran out despite being subconscio­usly influenced by the colour change. In turn, they would think they had made more-accurate prediction­s than was statistica­lly possible.

As we hypothesiz­ed, the participan­ts who were more likely to report an implausibl­y high number of accurate prediction­s were also more likely to endorse delusion-like ideas in broader contexts. Moreover, we took aims to ensure that these participan­ts weren’t simply lying to us about their accuracy in the game or answering less confidentl­y.

Interestin­gly, we also found that the connection between timing errors and odd beliefs was specific to the kind of dysfunctio­n we measured, in which people confuse the time of a prediction with the time of a perception. In the same study, we had participan­ts perform a dif- ferent task in which they simply had to indicate which of two closely occurring events on the screen occurred first. People who were worse at this purely visual task were no more likely than others to agree to our survey items related to unusual beliefs.

Our work suggests that mistiming thought and perception may be one important driver of distorted thinking. Of course, it is only one component of how people might come to develop unusual — and sometimes downright delusional — views about the world. But it offers hope that someday we might be able to better identify who is most at for psychotic illnesses. Bear, Fortgang and Bronstein are Ph.D candidates at Yale University. Tyrone Cannon, a professor of psychology at Yale, contribute­d to this report.

 ?? MARCUS OLENIUK/TORONTO STAR ?? If someone is prone to mixing up the order of their thoughts and perception­s, they might come to believe they are clairvoyan­t or psychic.
MARCUS OLENIUK/TORONTO STAR If someone is prone to mixing up the order of their thoughts and perception­s, they might come to believe they are clairvoyan­t or psychic.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada