Toronto Star

4 PRIMATES USING STONE TOOLS

Only a handful of non-humans have figured it out — including hungry capuchins,

- SARAH KAPLAN

For years, scientists at the Smithsonia­n Tropical Research Institute in Panama had whispered about the remote island where monkeys used stone tools. A botanist had witnessed the phenomenon during a long-ago survey — but, being more interested in flora than fauna at the time, she couldn’t linger to investigat­e. A return to the site would require new funds, good weather for a treacherou­s 56-kilometre boat ride, and days of swimming, hiking and camping amid rocky, wave-pounded shorelines and dense tropical forest.

“For a while, it kind of just stayed a rumour,” said Brendan Barrett, a behavioura­l ecologist at the Max Planck Institute for Ornitholog­y in Germany and a visiting researcher at the STRI. But when Barrett and his colleagues finally arrived at Jicaron Island in Panama’s Coiba National Park last year, what they found was well worth the effort: Tiny white-faced capuchin monkeys were using stones almost half their body weight as hammers to smash open shellfish, nuts and other foods.

“We were stunned,” said Barrett, the lead author of a new paper on the discovery posted on the preprint website bioRxiv.

The capuchins are the first animals of their genus observed using stone tools and only the fourth group of non-human primates known to do so. Sophistica­ted, social and tolerant of observatio­n, they also provide scientists with an ideal system for studying what causes a species to venture into the Stone Age — and could help researcher­s understand how and why our own ancestors first picked up stone tools more than two million years ago.

White-faced capuchins, or Cebus capucinus, are found all over the rain forests of Central America. Roughly the size of house cats, with nimble black bodies, long dexterous tails and expressive eyes, they live in matrilinea­l troops of about 20 animals and practise a variety of complex behaviours. They rub plants over their bodies, potentiall­y as medicine; defend themselves with sticks against snakes; play games by passing one another sticks and stones; and engage in weird “bond-testing” rituals that involve sticking their fingers in one another’s noses and eyes — perhaps the monkey equivalent of a trust fall.

A year of observing the inhabitant­s of Jicaron Island with dozens of motion-detecting cameras revealed that at least some of these animals are also talented at wielding stone tools. The monkeys were captured collecting large, heavy cobbles from streams and shorelines and carrying them to broad, flat rocks or logs that could be used as anvils. Standing on two feet, using their tails to anchor themselves against the ground or a nearby tree, they raised their “hammers” high above their heads and then smashed them down on nuts, crabs, snails and other foods — cracking open hard shells to reveal a tasty morsel.

The monkeys used their stone tools almost every day and often saved stones for repeated use. In one instance, a capuchin arrived at a stone tool site on the coast carrying an armful of almendro nuts, or “sea almonds.” He dropped them on the ground, then began bashing them open one by one.

Curiously, only male monkeys were seen wielding stone tools, even though females were often foraging nearby. This can’t be explained by females’ smaller size, given that juvenile males were able to use tools.

Even stranger, tool use was observed only among one group of capuchins occupying a roughly two-kilometre stretch of shoreline, even though Barrett and his colleagues surveyed the entirety of Jicaron as well as two nearby islands where the monkeys are also found. In a few cases, the scientists left “experiment­al” hammers and anvils in spots where no stone tools had been found; the monkeys ignored the artifacts, though a few Homo sapiens were observed using them to crack open coconuts.

Which makes Barrett wonder: Why hasn’t this seemingly significan­t behaviour spread across the island? What’s so special about this single group of tool users?

“That’s what makes this really interestin­g,” he said. “We’re in a position to actually make these comparison­s looking at why this would evolve.”

The paper hasn’t yet been published in a peerreview­ed journal, meaning it hasn’t been officially subjected to the scrutiny of other scientists. But Joan Silk, a primate behavioura­l ecologist at Arizona State University, said the study was a “careful and nice descriptiv­e analysis of this new observatio­n.”

“It’s just more good natural history to add to what we know about the way animals use tools in the wild,” she said. “And knowing more about tool use in other animals is super interestin­g because it helps us see how human tool use is different.”

Humans and our hominin cousins have been using stones as hammers and anvils for least three million years. Until recently, scientists thought we were the only creatures who did so.

But in the past few decades, chimpanzee­s have been observed using an entire “tool kit” — not just stone hammers but also shovel-like branches for drilling holes into termite nests and straw “fishing probes” to extract the tasty insects. On islands off Thailand, long-tailed macaque monkeys have become such capable tool users that they destabiliz­ed the local shellfish population.

And in South America, scientists have found hammer-and-anvil stones used by robust capuchin monkeys that date to at least 700 years ago. In fact, tool use was previously one of the traits used to distinguis­h that genus, Sapajus, from the smaller capuchins of the Cebus genus — including those in Panama.

That always seemed strange, Barrett said, because the Cebus monkeys of Coiba National Park — an archipelag­o of more than 100 islands on Panama’s Pacific coast — seemed like great candidates for new tool users.

For one thing, resources on the islands are restricted, so it’s advantageo­us to find new ways to access food. (Most of the world’s ablest tool users — the macaques of Thailand, New Caledonian crows — live on islands, Barrett noted.) The animals there have no natural predators, so they can afford to develop the loud, potentiall­y attention-grabbing habit of sitting on the ground and banging rocks together.

Much like humans, capuchins are “dietary generalist­s.” Rather than evolving a few physiologi­cal traits suited to certain kinds of foods — like powerful jaws for crushing nuts or big molars for chewing tough plants — “they can problem solve and get all kinds of things that way.”

Best of all, capuchins are fast learners, capable of picking up new feeding and social behaviours by watching other members of their species.

“They independen­tly evolved a huge reliance on culture,” Barrett said. “That makes them a really good comparison for human evolution.”

To strengthen that comparison, Barrett and his colleagues plan to broaden their surveys of the other islands in Coiba National Park and analyze whether the monkeys get an energetic benefit from using tools. They also aim to dig into Jicaron’s fossil record to see if they can uncover evidence of how this tool use began.

“Knowing more about tool use in other animals ... helps us see how human tool use is different.” JOAN SILK PRIMATE BEHAVIOURA­L ECOLOGIST

 ?? MICHAEL HASLAM/THE ASSOCIATED PRESS ?? In Panama, tiny white-faced capuchin monkeys were using stones almost half their body weight as hammers to smash open shellfish, nuts and other foods.
MICHAEL HASLAM/THE ASSOCIATED PRESS In Panama, tiny white-faced capuchin monkeys were using stones almost half their body weight as hammers to smash open shellfish, nuts and other foods.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada