Toronto Star

Sans Forgetica, the font designed to help you remember

- TAYLOR TELFORD

A new font can help lodge informatio­n deeper in your brain, researcher­s say, but it’s not magic — just the science of effort.

Psychology and design researcher­s at RMIT University in Melbourne created a font called Sans Forgetica, which was designed to boost informatio­n retention for readers. It’s based on a theory called “desirable difficulty,” which suggests that people remember things better when their brains have to overcome minor obstacles while processing informatio­n. Sans Forgetica is sleek and backslante­d with intermitte­nt gaps in each letter, which serve as a “simple puzzle” for the reader, according to Stephen Banham, a designer and RMIT lecturer who helped create the font.

“It should be difficult to read but not too difficult,” Banham said. “In demanding this additional act, memory is more likely to be triggered.”

In designing Sans Forgetica, Banham said he had to override his instincts, ingrained from 25 years of studying typography. Clarity, the ease of processing and familiarit­y are usually guiding principles in the field. The back-slanting in Sans Forgetica would be foreign to most readers, as back-slanting in type is typically only used by cartograph­ers to indicate rivers. The openings in the letters make the brain pause to identify the shapes.

The team tested the font’s efficacy along with other intentiona­lly complicate­d fonts on 400 students in lab and online experiment­s and found that “Sans Forgetica broke just enough design principles without becoming too illegible and aided memory retention,” according to a news release on the university’s website.

In some ways, Sans Forgetica is a continuati­on of work by Daniel Oppenheime­r, a Carnegie Mellon psychology professor who presented a similar idea about “desirable difficulty” called “disfluency” while doing work at Princeton University in 2011. The terms are different, but the principle is the same: minor mental gymnastics help readers remember things better.

Oppenheime­r’s team relied on contrast and size for the mental hurdles rather than on a foreign font. In one experiment, the team had 28 college stu- dents read informatio­n about two made-up creatures, the pangerish and the norgletti. The pangerish informatio­n was printed in grey, 12-point Comic Sans or Bodoni fonts. The norgletti profile was printed in 16-point, plain, black Arial font.

The team distracted students for 15 minutes after they read about the animals, then quizzed them; the students remembered 87 per cent of the pangerish facts, whose informatio­n had been tougher to read, and 73 per cent of the norgletti facts.

“This research shows that behavioura­l interventi­ons can be an important element in school reform,” Oppenheime­r said in an interview with Harvard Business Review.

Janneke Blijlevens, another researcher on the project, stressed that the font should be used sparingly for it to stay effective. If the reader’s brain gets too comfortabl­e, it will glaze over Sans Forgetica just as easily as if it were Arial or Times New Roman.

 ?? THE WASHINGTON POST ?? Sans Forgetica was designed by researcher­s as a reading retention tool.
THE WASHINGTON POST Sans Forgetica was designed by researcher­s as a reading retention tool.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada