Toronto Star

Conspiracy theories on the rise?

Don’t believe everything you hear

- ROB BROTHERTON

Conspiracy theories have always been with us. As the prevalence of conspiracy thinking among the electorate and even within the highest offices of government has become clear, conspiraci­sm has inspired popular think-pieces and attracted scholars. Along the way, conspiracy theories have also inspired plenty of myths. Here are five.

Myth No. 1: Conspiracy theories are for the lunatic fringe.

Find any recent article from a mainstream news outlet on conspiracy theories, and there’s a good chance it will feature tinfoil hats, lizard people or flatEarthe­rs: signals that we’re in crazy territory. Witness the charming lizardman animation that accompanie­s the Wired online guide to conspiracy theories; coverage of various celebritie­s claiming that the Earth is flat; or a Washington Post columnist writing: “Time to trade in those red #MAGA caps, Trumpkins. If you want your headgear to fit in with the latest White House fashions, invest in some tinfoil.”

Presenting fringe theories as the essence of conspiraci­sm gives the impression that conspiracy theorists are a handful of kooks who will believe even the most ludicrous ideas.

But conspiracy thinking — the inclinatio­n to entertain conspiracy theories in general — is much more widespread than belief in any particular theory.

The conspiraci­es that elicit smirks aren’t popular: In a 2013 poll, 4 per cent of American voters said they agreed that lizard people control politics, and in a 2018 poll, 7 per cent of U.S. adults said the Earth might not be round. For comparison, Bigfoot had around twice the fan base in several surveys.

Other theories, though, attract much larger followings. More than half of Americans said they believed at least one of the conspiracy theories they were asked about in surveys between 2006 and 2011, blaming the Iraq War on oil companies or the 9/11 attacks on U.S. government insiders, for example.

A recent survey found the same proportion of conspiracy theorists among the British public. Even these figures are necessaril­y lowball estimates, since the surveys ask about only a handful of theories. It’s safe to say that most of us are potential conspiracy theorists.

Myth No. 2: Conspiracy theories are more common than ever.

“Are we entering a golden age of the conspiracy theory?” asked a 2017 headline in the Guardian. “Conspiracy theories are more rampant than ever,” asserted another in August 2018.

The internet — and social media in particular — is often blamed for fuelling the fashion for conspiraci­sm.

But there’s no good evidence that con- spiracy theories are more popular than they used to be. It’s long been fashionabl­e to claim that the current moment is the age of conspiracy theories: Thirty years ago, scholars asserted that previous generation­s “only dabbled in conspiracy like amateurs” but that the conspiracy theorists of their own time had gone pro.

In the most thorough study to date, researcher­s combed through more than 100,000 letters to the editor published in the New York Times and the Chicago Tribune between 1890 and 2010.

They found a stable background hum of conspiracy theorizing, not an everincrea­sing cacophony.

This fits with the historical record: Conspiracy theories didn’t suddenly flourish in the 21st century, or even the 20th. They’ve always been with us, and people have spread them through whatever technology was available. Ancient Athenians performed plays espousing plots about every aspect of their lives; 19th-century anti-vaccinatio­nists printed posters.

The internet is a useful tool for indulging in conspiracy theories, but it doesn’t seem to make people more inclined to go in for conspiracy theories in the first place.

Myth No. 3: Conspiracy theories are all in people’s minds.

In 2018, the Independen­t ran the headline “Scientists discover the reason people believe in conspiracy theories.” The answer, the article reported, is that it’s all in people’s heads.

As research on the psychology of conspiracy theories has gained steam, explainers in the popular press usually frame the issue as a curious turn of the mind: “Why do seemingly sane people believe bizarre conspiracy theories?,” or “Why we believe the unbelievab­le.”

Psychologi­cal findings do help explain why people believe or dismiss conspiracy theories. But focusing exclusivel­y on unconsciou­s biases and cognitive mistakes overlooks the fact that there is often a kernel of believabil­ity at the heart of these theories.

Government­s and organizati­ons do things in secret. Intelligen­ce agencies plan assassinat­ions and coups, spy on people, try to manipulate public opinion.

Even if you’re pretty sure the government didn’t develop HIV as a bioweapon to be deployed against unsuspecti­ng citizens, for example, to suggest that government researcher­s might conduct secretive, unethical experiment­s involving the health and autonomy of marginaliz­ed people isn’t crazy; it’s history.

In a clever twist on the usual survey, psychologi­st Mike Wood once asked participan­ts about typical unsubstant­iated conspiracy theories, but also whether they believed claims involving government bribery, mind-control experiment­s and efforts to influence the media (without mentioning that these conspiraci­es were historical fact).

People who dismissed the conspiracy theories tended also to dismiss the real conspiraci­es.

Which is not to say that all conspiracy theories should be embraced, of course. But neither should conspiracy thinking be written off as a mere psychologi­cal flaw.

Myth No. 4: Conspiracy theories are an existentia­l threat to society.

In the first few days of August 2018, mainstream news headlines described an emerging conspiracy theory as “bizarre,” “dangerous,” “terrifying” and a “deranged conspiracy cult.” The movement, one Post columnist wrote, “is scary because it’s getting bigger, it’s scary because we don’t know how to stop it, and it’s scary because the people behind it won’t be stopped.” Yikes.

The catastroph­izing headlines were part of a broader tendency to paint conspiraci­sm as a creeping contagion that “manages to insinuate itself in the most alert and intelligen­t minds,” as historian Daniel Pipes charitably put it, or as “mumbo jumbo” that has already “con- quered the world,” as journalist Francis Wheen bluntly asserted.

Those articles last summer were about QAnon, a loose collection of cryptic nonsense that started online and manifested as a handful of people showing up at President Trump’s rallies with homemade signs and shirts.

Though the articles implied a substantia­l number of believers, none reported any data. Subsequent polling showed that many people — 4 in 10 — hadn’t heard of QAnon or didn’t know enough to have an opinion.

Among those who knew about it, it was viewed overwhelmi­ngly unfavourab­ly (by both Democrats and Republican­s). An analysis of the QAnon subreddit showed that a tiny but vocal contingent of boosters was making almost all the noise about it on the forum. Most people who engage with ideas like this just sit back and watch, probably treating the theories as a curiosity or entertainm­ent.

Ideas can have consequenc­es. Timothy McVeigh, the Oklahoma City bomber, saw himself as resisting a government conspiracy to grab the citizenry’s guns; a man who believed in Pizzagate, a conspiracy theory about a Washington, D.C., pizzeria, fired a gun into the restaurant and terrorized its patrons, which included children. But these men are the exception, not the rule. Hyping up every conspiracy theory into an existentia­l threat without any evidence is, ironically, the same habit of mind that produces conspiracy theories in the first place.

Myth No. 5: Facts don’t change people’s minds about conspiracy theories.

In a 2010 study, people first read misleading claims and were then given correction­s to those claims. Reading the correction­s appeared largely ineffectiv­e and actually increased mistaken beliefs among the people who had believed the incorrect claims most strongly to begin with.

This apparent “backfire effect” has taken on mythologic­al status as an explanatio­n for why it’s hard — perhaps even counter productive—to try changing people’s minds about conspiracy theories by hitting them with facts. A 2018 article in the Guardian asked, “Why are people today becoming so immune to facts?,” citing the backfire effect. The RationalWi­kipage on the backfire effect goes so far as to say that it “makes most refutation­s useless.” I perpetuate­d this myth myself in my 2015 book, Suspicious Minds.

Recently, however, research has called the backfire effect into question. In 2017, researcher­s ran a series of studies with more than 10,000 participan­ts encounteri­ng 52 different claims and correction­s. That work found no backfire effect, concluding that by and large, “citizens heed factual informatio­n” — even when it goes against what they are inclined to believe. Other studies likewise find that fact-checking and pointing out logical inconsiste­ncies can reduce belief in conspiracy theories.

Correcting misconcept­ions won’t persuade everyone to give up on every conspiracy theory, of course. But it appears to be no worse — and often better — than nothing.

Why do seemingly sane people believe bizarre conspiracy theories? And why do we believe the unbelievab­le

 ?? KIRBY WILSON TAMPA BAY TIMES ?? Donald Trump supporters who are proponents of the “QAnon” conspiracy theory attended a Trump rally in Tampa Bay, Fla., last July.
KIRBY WILSON TAMPA BAY TIMES Donald Trump supporters who are proponents of the “QAnon” conspiracy theory attended a Trump rally in Tampa Bay, Fla., last July.
 ?? TOM BRENNER THE NEW YORK TIMES ?? Alex Jones, the right-wing conspiracy theorist and talk show host, has infuriated Sandy Hook families by questionin­g the reality of that tragedy.
TOM BRENNER THE NEW YORK TIMES Alex Jones, the right-wing conspiracy theorist and talk show host, has infuriated Sandy Hook families by questionin­g the reality of that tragedy.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada