Toronto Star

Who watches terror videos? Jobless men, survey finds

- ISAAC STANLEY-BECKER

Roxane Cohen Silver began trying to understand how graphic images pollute the human psyche in the terror-struck days after the 9/11 attacks, when sights and sounds of the twin towers crumbling flooded newspapers, television and radio.

The latest work by the professor of psychologi­cal science at the University of California at Irvine arrives in the midst of a reckoning with another terrorist attack. The massacre of 50 people at two mosques in Christchur­ch, New Zealand, occurred halfway around the globe from Ground Zero, but on a whole other planet of opportunit­ies to view extremist violence.

In 2001, the images of billowing smoke got their oxygen almost exclusivel­y from legacy media. In 2019, the rat-a-tat of gunfire issuing from semi-automatic weapons — and footage of the bloodshed inflicted by the weaponry — became available live on Facebook and other social media platforms.

The grisly events in New Zealand, which reignited debate about the ethics of viewing terrorist propaganda and the responsibi­lities of technology companies to curtail its circulatio­n, occurred soon after Silver’s newest paper appeared in the American Psychologi­st, the flagship journal of the American Psychologi­cal Associatio­n. Her results, published late last month, hold clues about what sort of people are drawn to these images of destructio­n, as well as the psychologi­cal toll the gruesome material may take.

The study, “Who Watches an ISIS Beheading — and Why,” examines engagement with videos depicting the decapitati­on of journalist­s James Foley and Steven Sotloff by Daesh militants. The videos, each several minutes in length, appeared on the internet about three weeks apart in 2014.

They shocked the world, enlisting modern methods to achieve barbaric ends. But the paper is the first to quantify how many people actually watched this graphic content, who these people were and what their motivation­s were for doing so.

In a survey of more than 3,000 U.S. residents that began in 2013 and lasted for three years, Silver and her colleagues at UCI found that 20 per cent had watched at least part of a beheading video, while five per cent said they had watched an entire one.

Survey participan­ts were more likely to say they had watched the beheading videos if they also said they were male, Christian or unemployed. Those who reported watching television more often were also more likely to view the videos. So, too, was fear of future terrorism associated with greater likelihood of watching. Prior mental health conditions were not significan­t factors, but a lifetime exposure to violence was.

Age mattered only slightly. Yet, contrary to the widespread belief that young people are more prone to perusing the dark corners of the internet, Silver said, the tendency to watch the videos actually increased with age.

Most likely to engage with graphic content, Silver has found, are those who are already afraid of what they think they might encounter. The content has its desired effect, the paper concludes, increasing distress and fear of future negative events.

When we subject ourselves to images, sounds and videos created and distribute­d by terrorists, said Silver, who is the paper’s senior author, “We’re doing terrorists’ job for them.”

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada