Windsor Star

ONLY YOUNG ONCE

Animal adolescenc­e is filled with teen drama and peer pressure, but there’s a purpose

- DUNCAN STRAUSS

There are no reported sightings of surly teenage elephants reluctantl­y sitting down at the family dinner table, trusty ear buds in place, occasional­ly trumpeting monosyllab­ic answers.

But adolescent elephants do exhibit other behaviours many parents of human teens would recognize, said Cynthia Moss, a researcher who has studied and written books about elephants in Kenya’s Amboseli National Park for nearly five decades.

“They’re naive, they have a lot to learn and they make mistakes,” Moss said.

This is particular­ly true for males, she explained: They raid crops. They get speared. They die.

“It’s just like young human males who drive too fast,” she said, “and the insurance companies know very well to make them pay higher insurance rates.”

These sorts of low-judgment, high-risk actions, and many other youthful traits that traverse species, are explored in a recent book, Wildhood: The Epic Journey From Adolescenc­e to Adulthood in Humans and Other Animals by Barbara Natterson-horowitz, a Harvard evolutiona­ry biologist, and Kathryn Bowers, a science journalist.

The authors make clear that, in a fundamenta­l sense, adolescent animals and teen humans encounter the same sorts of challenges — and that stage of developmen­t is truly valuable. Other scientists who have studied adolescent­s — human and non-human — echo their findings.

Human “adolescent­s frequently put themselves in danger deliberate­ly,” Natterson-horowitz and Bowers write, adding: “Adolescent risk-taking is seen throughout the animal world.”

One reason is that they engage in behaviours that are risky but beneficial, said Bowers. An example is

a practice called “predator inspection,” or approachin­g predators rather than fleeing. The trade-off is that adolescent animals watch, smell and learn, accumulati­ng all kinds of informatio­n that can keep them safer as adults.

“The idea that adolescent­s are hardwired to take these risks can put a new spin on the knucklehea­ded antics of our own human teens,” Bowers said.

There’s also considerab­le time spent roving in battalions — marked by peak levels of peer pressure — and flirting with disaster. Indeed, scientists have documented and observed that adolescent­s of all stripes are more inclined to make perilous moves while with peers.

Laurence Steinberg, a psychology professor at Temple University, worked on two related studies — one involving mice, half of which were adolescent­s, drinking ethanol-spiked water, and another in

which human teens played a video game that reproduced driving conditions. The results were strikingly similar.

“We found that in the presence of peers, adolescent mice drank more than they do when they’re alone,” said Steinberg, who has published several books on adolescenc­e. “But we didn’t find any such peer effects in adults, which is identical to the kinds of things we were finding in human experiment­s.”

Steinberg said the teenagers in the simulated driving study also took more risks when others were around, regardless of whether they interacted with their peers. These findings dovetail with what Steinberg says is another multispeci­es adolescent hallmark: the desire to socialize. Anyone who has seen a pod of dolphins swimming alongside a boat zipping across the sea or watched videos of dolphins interactin­g with one another might

conclude that these playful critters are highly social. Ann Weaver, an animal behaviouri­st who studies bottlenose dolphins in the intercosta­l waterway off the coast of St. Petersburg, Florida, would agree.

“We just got off the water,” Weaver said in a recent interview. “We were out there for about three hours, and almost all the dolphins we saw were small gangs of teens.”

Asked to compare the behaviours of dolphin gangs and gaggles of teenage people, Weaver thought for a moment.

“They are at their most physically fit, they’re the strongest they’re going to be and they do everything with exaggerati­on,” she said, adding that she considers adolescenc­e on the water “a contact sport.”

Dolphins are featured in Wildhood, in which Natterson-horowitz and Bowers go deep and wide in addressing the raft of species-spanning equivalent­s. But they focus on

four individual animals — a king penguin, a spotted hyena, a humpback whale and a grey wolf — as they advance through adolescenc­e. And this voyage, the authors find, hinges on mastering four fundamenta­l skills: staying safe, negotiatin­g social status, navigating sexuality and living as adults. As moored in science as their book is, Natterson-horowitz and Bowers acknowledg­e it was partly shaped by their personal experience. Each was raising a human adolescent while working on the book.

“Having an adolescent at home, with all the drama and the ups and downs, particular­ly the moodiness — and social media was just coming into the picture — guided us in terms of what we wanted to look for, what we wanted to better understand in the wild to bring back to our homes,” Natterson-horowitz said.

For The Washington Post

 ??  ?? Adolescent elephants are “naive, they have a lot to learn and they make mistakes,” said researcher Cynthia Moss, who has studied elephants in Kenya for almost five decades.
BONNIE JO MOUNT/THE WASHINGTON POST
Adolescent elephants are “naive, they have a lot to learn and they make mistakes,” said researcher Cynthia Moss, who has studied elephants in Kenya for almost five decades. BONNIE JO MOUNT/THE WASHINGTON POST

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Canada