DT Next

Spectacle sports: Why these Mexican fish do the wave

- VERONICA GREENWOOD Greenwood is a journalist with NYT©2021

In the sulfur-infused ponds of Tabasco State in Mexico lives a tiny silver slip of a fish, the sulphur molly. Toss in a rock, and you might see a bunch of them dance: The water’s surface will erupt in pale, pulsing waves, spreading through the eerie blue like milk through coffee. Every few seconds, thousands of fish will repeat a quick diving motion to generate the wave, sometimes for up to two minutes. Why? biologists asked. What purpose could this flashing serve? The mollies are prey for an array of winged predators, including egrets, kingfisher­s and kiskadees. When birds dive to attack, the mollies flash and swirl.

Scientists in Germany, unable to visit the fish because of the coronaviru­s pandemic, analysed hours of video taken over the course of two years of bird attacks, both real and simulated by a researcher, and believe they may have decoded the missive being transmitte­d by the fish. It seems to be aimed at predators perched on the shore, they report in Current

Biology on Wednesday. The message reads: We see you. We are watching. Don’t try any funny business. Not every bird attack triggers the uncanny flashing, said David Bierbach, a biologist at the Leibniz-Institute of Freshwater Ecology and Inland Fisheries, and an author of the new paper. Kingfisher­s, for instance, cannonball into the water and provoke the mollies into flashing nearly every time. But kiskadees are subtle — they dip just their beaks in. They rarely set off a response.

This observatio­n gave the researcher­s a way to test their hypothesis that the flashing could result in a change in the predators’ behavior. They set up perches along a sulfur stream as well as cameras to film hunting kiskadees. After a bird had made one pass over the water, a researcher with a slingshot triggered the waving behavior in the fish, imitating something the kiskadees saw routinely when a kingfisher was hunting alongside them. Now they could compare undisturbe­d hunting and disturbed hunting. As the fish rippled and seethed, the kiskadees sat on the perches nearby. Over the course of more than 200 hunting sessions, the researcher­s saw that the birds waited twice as long before starting another pass than when the water remained undisturbe­d.

When they did attack again, they were much less successful at catching a fish than with still waters. Without the interferen­ce of the researcher­s, the birds caught a fish more than half the time. With the slingshot in play, it was less than a quarter of the time. When the researcher­s watched kingfisher­s, they saw that the more times the fish flashed, the longer the birds waited, as if they were waiting it out, too.

That response suggests that not only does the flashing behavior make it harder for the predator to zero in on a fish, but the birds also know that their efforts are more likely to be wasted once the waves begin. This is an intriguing observatio­n, because if the fish were merely trying to escape from the predators, they could dive deeper and stay down longer. Though the low-oxygen environmen­t of a sulfur pool means they can’t stay under indefinite­ly, they are perfectly capable of more extended stays below, Dr. Bierbach said.

“They can stay for up to two minutes or three minutes under the water,” he said. “But they don’t do it. They come quickly back up to the surface and repeat their diving, very synchronou­sly, very rhythmical­ly.” Synchronis­ed behavior, like in swarms of fireflies that blink in unison or flocks of birds that move together in a carefully spaced pattern across the sky, has long fascinated scientists and anyone else who is lucky enough to have seen it. But so far, it has proved difficult to pinpoint exactly what benefit the creatures receive from it and why it might have evolved.

 ?? ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from India