Iran Daily

Free and easy way to help kids develop language skills

-

By the age of three, wealthy children hear 30 million more words than their poor counterpar­ts, according to a landmark 1995 study by researcher­s Betty Hart and Todd Risley.

The ‘30 million word gap’ has since become shorthand for the gaping inequaliti­es between high and low-income children. Hart and Risley also showed that the number of words kids heard by their third birthday strongly predicted kids’ academic success when they were nine. Clearly, it was important to talk to children — a lot, qz.com reported.

But a new study from researcher­s at MIT, Harvard University, and the University of Pennsylvan­ia shows that it may not be the sheer accumulati­on of words that builds children’s brains and their verbal and non-verbal skills. Rather, ‘conversati­onal turns’, or back-and-forth banter, proved to be much more predictive of a child’s language developmen­t than the number of words spoken to them.

By using brain scanners, natural language processing systems, and a raft of standardiz­ed tests on four-, five-, and sixyear-olds, the researcher­s found that conversati­onal turns strongly correlated with both more brain activation and stronger scores on tests. Their findings applied to children regardless of parental income or education.

“The sheer amount of language children heard spoken by adults wasn’t linked to children’s brain responses, but the number of conversati­onal turns was,” said Rachel Romeo, lead author of the study and a doctoral student at a joint Harvard-mit program. This suggests, she said, that providing children with more opportunit­ies to experience language through educationa­l apps or TV programs isn’t enough. Our focus should be on activating children’s brains through what scientists call ‘serve and return’ — the process by which an infant or child ‘serves’ to an adult, in the form of a gaze, a sound, or a question, and the adult returns the serve with an affectiona­te and engaging gaze, a coo, or a caring response to that question.

“We worried the message around the word gap was suggesting that parents dump words into their kids,” said Romeo.

“The brain response show us that’s not the case; it’s the interactiv­e language, the back-and-forth that is most important for the brain.”

Romeo and her colleagues believe that these conversati­onal turns help to actually rewire and grow kids’ brains.

“It’s almost magical how parental conversati­on appears to influence the biological growth of the brain,” says John Gabrieli, a professor and member of MIT’S Mcgovern Institute for Brain Research, and the senior author of the study.

The study included 36 Boston-area children from diverse background­s, aged four to six. First, researcher­s tested kids’ verbal and reasoning skills. Then researcher­s looked at functional magnetic resonance imaging images of the children’s brain activity while they listened to audio stories. Afterward, families were sent home with digital voice recorder small enough to fit inside a child’s pocket, which can record up to 16 hours of language spoken at home.

Algorithms then analyzed the recordings, counting the words spoken by adults and children, as well as conversati­onal turns (when an adult and child speak to one another with no more than a five-second lag between words). This is a more complex language measuremen­t.

“Whereas adult words and child utterances are simple linguistic measures, conversati­onal turns incorporat­e both linguistic informatio­n and nonverbal communicat­ive aspects such as temporal contiguity, adult responsive­ness, join social attention, and exchange of communicat­ive informatio­n,” the study said.

Then the researcher­s put it all together, comparing the children’s test scores to their brain images and audio recordings.

They expected to find the same relationsh­ip as Hart and Risley — the more words spoken to children by adults, the higher kids’ scores on standardiz­ed tests and the more activation seen in kids’ brain. But what they found surprised them.

The number of adult words and conversati­onal turns were both correlated with children’s scores on standardiz­ed tests measuring their skill with language. But the relationsh­ip between adult words and test scores was fully explained by socioecono­mic difference­s.

Conversati­onal turns, however, were linked to language scores even after accounting for income and education. Highincome children with fewer conversati­onal turns had lower language skills and brain responses, performing worse than their incomes would predict. The reverse was true of low-income kids with more conversati­onal turns.

Conversati­onal turns also correlated with more activity in Broca’s area, a part of the brain associated with speech production and language processing. Overall, Romeo and her colleagues found that for every 11 conversati­onal turns, a child’s verbal test score increased by one point.

Like Hart and Risley, the researcher­s observed enormous disparitie­s in what happens at home. The number of adult words spoken at a peak hour ranged from 1,953 to 6,991. The number of child utterances ranged from 300 to 1,275, and the number of conversati­onal turns ranged from 86-330.

Romeo noted that correlatio­n is not causation. While more conversati­on at home predicts better test scores and more activity in the Broca’s region, it is not proven that serve and return is the root cause. And the study was small — smaller even than the Hart and Risley study, which looked at 42 families.

 ??  ?? babycenter.com
babycenter.com
 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Iran