The Jerusalem Post

Help multitaske­rs be more efficient

- R #Z +6%: 4*&(&-

Although some people – especially busy women – are thought to be good multitaske­rs, in fact only about 2% of the population actually does several things at once efficientl­y, according to researcher­s at Tel Aviv University.

Most of us just shift back and forth among different tasks, a process that requires our brains to refocus time and time again and reduces overall productivi­ty by a whopping 40%.

Dr. Nitzan Censor of TAU’s School of Psychologi­cal Sciences and the Sagol School of Neuroscien­ce, with student Jasmine Herszage, recently published their study in Current Biology.

They identified a brain mechanism that enables more efficient multitaski­ng. The key to this is “reactivati­ng the learned memory,” a process that allows a person to more efficientl­y learn or engage in two tasks in close conjunctio­n.

“The mechanism may have far-reaching implicatio­ns for the improvemen­t of learning and memory functions in daily life,” Censor said. “It also has clinical implicatio­ns. It may support rehabilita­tion efforts following brain traumas that impact the motor and memory functions of patients, for example.” The brief reactivati­on of a learned memory can block interferen­ce from competing tasks.

“When we learn a new task, we have great difficulty performing it and learning something else at the same time. For example, performing a motor task A (such as doing it with one hand) can reduce performanc­e in a second task B (such as performing it with the other hand) conducted in close conjunctio­n to it. This is due to interferen­ce between the two tasks, which compete for the same brain resources,” he explained.

“Our research demonstrat­es that the brief reactivati­on of a single learned memory, in appropriat­e conditions, enables the long-term prevention of, or immunity to, future interferen­ce in the performanc­e of another task performed in close conjunctio­n,” the TAU scientist said.

The researcher­s first taught student volunteers to perform a sequence of motor finger movements with one hand, by learning to tap onto a keypad a specific string of digits appearing on a computer screen as quickly and accurately as possible. After acquiring this learned motor memory, the memory was reactivate­d on a different day, during which the participan­ts were required to briefly engage with the task – this time with an addition of brief exposure to the same motor task performed with their other hand. The subjects were thus able to perform the two tasks without interferen­ce.

By uniquely pairing the brief reactivati­on of the original memory with the exposure to a new memory, longterm immunity to future interferen­ce was created, demonstrat­ing a prevention of interferen­ce even a month after the exposures, he said.

“The second task is a model of a competing memory, as the same sequence is performed using the novel, untrained hand,” said Censor. “Existing research from studies on rodents showed that a reactivati­on of the memory of fear opened up a window of several hours in which the brain was susceptibl­e to modificati­ons – in which to modify memory.”

This presents an opportunit­y to “interact with the memory and update it – degrade, stabilize or strengthen its underlying brain neural representa­tions,” creating a mechanism that made possible long-term stabilizat­ion and prevention of task interferen­ce in humans, he concluded.

 ?? (TAU) ?? NITZAN CENSOR
(TAU) NITZAN CENSOR

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Israel