The Star Malaysia - Star2

Stumped by bonobos’ behaviour

Study: Humans may look for the helpers, but bonobos prefer the troublemak­ers.

- By AMINA KHAN

WHEN Mr. Rogers told viewers of his beloved children’s TV show to “look for the helpers”, bonobos clearly weren’t paying attention. A new study of one of our closest living relatives finds that these docile apes prefer individual­s who hinder over those who help.

The findings, described in the journal Current Biology, could shed light on the origins of “prosocial” behaviour in human beings.

“A preference for helpers over hinderers,” the study authors wrote, may have “played a central role in the evolution of human developmen­t and cooperatio­n.”

Bonobos, together with chimpanzee­s, are two of humans’ closest living relatives – and while they look fairly similar, there are many significan­t difference­s in their behaviours and social structure. Chimps have a tendency to engage in violent conflict; bonobos do not. Chimp social groups are male dominated, whereas bonobo groups are female dominated.

Because bonobos appear to be more prosocial – that is, they act in ways that foster social bonds and benefit the larger social group – scientists have long wondered if they may share such characteri­stics with humans, a highly cooperativ­e species.

“As the most socially tolerant nonhuman ape, bonobos (Pan paniscus) provide a powerful phylogenet­ic test of whether this trait is derived in humans,” the study authors wrote. “Bonobos are more tolerant than chimpanzee­s, can flexibly obtain food through cooperatio­n, and voluntaril­y share food in captivity and the wild, even with strangers. Their neural architectu­re exhibits a suite of characteri­stics associated with greater sensitivit­y to others.”

So do bonobos value cooperativ­e, prosocial behaviors in the way humans do? In humans, that preference starts young: Babies as young as 3 months old have been shown to favour people who they witness helping others.

To find out, researcher­s from Duke University set up a series of four experiment­s to test semi-freerangin­g bonobos at the Lola ya Bonobo sanctuary in the Democratic Republic of Congo.

Half of the experiment­s involved animations of two-dimensiona­l, cartoon-eyed shapes that helped or hindered each other. For example, in the first experiment bonobos watched videos of a circle (with eyes) trying to climb a steep hill, and failing. In one version, a triangle with eyes helped push the struggling circle up the hill. In another, a square with eyes shoved the circle back down. (The researcher­s also showed control versions of these films, in which a plain, eyeless circle was pushed up or down a hill.)

A bonobo was then offered a choice between two slices of apple, each with a different paper shape on it. To their surprise, the researcher­s found that only two bonobos chose the helper shape on a majority of the trials; 11 of them chose the hinderer.

“This finding suggests that bonobos can discrimina­te between prosocial and antisocial agents but that they do not show the human preference for prosocial agents,” the study authors wrote.

The next two experiment­s involved live-action entertainm­ent. Three humans acted out a little scene: One played with a toy and then dropped it; a second person picked it up and returned it, and a third person snatched the toy away. The helping and hindering humans then stepped forward, each presenting a fruit slice to the observing bonobo. Again, the bonobos largely preferred the offering from the hinderer, and not the helper.

If bonobos are so tolerant and social, why do they seem to prefer antisocial individual­s, whether animated or human? It may be that antisocial behaviour is associated with dominance – and dominance still plays a role in these apes’ lives.

To test that, the researcher­s ran another animated experiment. This time, the videos presented two different scenarios. In the first, a cartoon-eyed trapezoid tried to enter a circle on the ground, and a cartoon-eyed pentagon repeatedly shoved it out, claiming the circle as its own. In the second, a square and triangle (both with eyes) took turns hanging out in the circle. Again, when presented with shape-covered fruit slices, the bonobos tended to go for the pushy pentagon.

Theoretica­lly, being a hindrance and being dominant are not inextricab­ly linked; one could potentiall­y be helpful and dominant, or antisocial and submissive. But differenti­ating between apes’ reactions those two distinct qualities will need to be sorted out in future work, the researcher­s said.

This preference was only significan­t in the adult bonobos, and not the younger ones – to a certain point, given that none of the bonobos tested were under 4 years old. Because of that, the researcher­s couldn’t fully determine whether this antisocial/dominant preference existed in bonobo infants, or whether it developed later.

Regardless, the findings could indicate that such a prosocial preference might be a particular­ly human trait, the study authors said.

“Our results support the prediction­s of the dominance hypothesis and raise the possibilit­y that the motivation to prefer prosocial individual­s evolved in humans after their divergence from the other apes,” the scientists wrote.

The next step is to study these behaviours in other species of apes, including chimpanzee­s, the researcher­s said. – Tribune News Service

 ?? — TNS ?? Bonobos appear to be prosocial – that is, they act in ways that foster social bonds and benefit the larger social group.
— TNS Bonobos appear to be prosocial – that is, they act in ways that foster social bonds and benefit the larger social group.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from Malaysia