Manawatu Standard

Rhinos ‘Facebook’ with dung deposits

Dogs communicat­e with urine and it turns out that other species communicat­e with dung. Amina Khan reports.

-

Here’s a social networking site you can find with your nose. Scientists studying the communal pooping areas of white rhinos in South Africa have found that the animals use them like social messaging boards – to leave notes about their status and to read the ‘‘posts’’ of others.

The discovery, published in the Proceeding­s of the Royal Society B, offers insight into the behaviours of these and other social animals – and could even help researcher­s develop new tools to aid in their conservati­on.

Mammals are known to leave one another scent signals in their urine (as anyone watching a dog studiously sniff a lamppost while his owner tugs on the leash might suspect). But the potential signalsend­ing power of poo has garnered relatively less attention, scientists said.

‘‘Urine has been more heavily studied,’’ lead author Courtney Marneweck, an ecologist and doctoral student at the University of Kwazulu-natal in South Africa, said ‘‘Perhaps because the behaviour of urine marking is so obvious (that is, it normally comes via urine spraying), its function is obviously more than pure eliminatio­n. Because specific behaviours are less common when defecating, perhaps the communicat­ory significan­ce was unrealised.’’

But scientists have begun to suspect that dung may actually play an important role in social communicat­ion – especially for animals like the Oribi antelope, the coyote and the white rhinoceros, which uses communal defecation sites (also known as ‘‘middens’’).

‘‘From behavioura­l observatio­ns we were really sure that rhinos were using middens to gather informatio­n about each other,’’ Marneweck said. ‘‘This was first noticed by [South African researcher] Norman Owen-smith in the early 1970s. But only recent developmen­ts in technology have allowed us to really understand odours.’’

There were several reasons white rhinos were the perfect study subjects, Marneweck said.

‘‘White rhinos are a great model species to study olfactory communicat­ion (that’s communicat­ion via smell),’’ she wrote. ‘‘Firstly, they have very bad eyesight and rely on olfaction heavily. Second, the middens that they use are large (up to 20 metres in diameter), frequent, and really easy to find.’’

The plan was to gather dung immediatel­y after a rhinoceros relieved itself. That way researcher­s would know exactly which individual deposited each sample, which would allow them to figure out whether there were any telltale chemicals that correlated with that animal’s identifyin­g characteri­stics (like sex or age).

Finding these smelly, sizeable middens may have been easy – but catching a rhino in the act was no walk in the park, she added.

‘‘Field work is always challengin­g, but collecting all this data took a long time ... I think my record for waiting for a rhino to poo was 7.5 hours,’’ Marneweck wrote. ‘‘That’s after walking and tracking it!’’

After sampling the volatile, or airborne, chemicals from 150 dung samples, she used a patternrec­ognition algorithm to figure out the ‘‘smell profiles’’ of each animal in terms of sex, age, territoria­l state (for males) and whether they were in heat (for females).

(For those who are curious: According to the study, 2,3-dimethylun­decane signalled an animal’s sex, heptanal revealed its age group, nonane indicated a male’s territoria­l status, and 2,6-dimethylun­decane revealed whether a female was in heat.)

Once she and her colleagues knew which airborne chemicals correlated with each characteri­stic, Marneweck created a faux deposit for a territoria­l male and a female in heat (known as an ‘‘oestrous female’’) and put them in the field to see what effect they had in the wild.

Sure enough, when a territoria­l male smelled the oestrous female, he sniffed the source for a long time and then went to track her down. But if he smelled a territoria­l rival, he immediatel­y became vigilant and then tried to search out the threat. (Smelling non-dominant males and nonoestrou­s females didn’t seem to matter to the territoria­l male.)

The findings show that rhinoceros­es really do leave very specific ‘‘posts’’ about themselves at these middens that help drive the local population’s social activity.

There’s a good reason to use urine and dung to deliver these messages, Marneweck pointed out – it doesn’t cost any extra energy to do so.

‘‘Using a waste product for a scent marking signal is energetica­lly smart,’’ she said. ‘‘It is costly for animals to create specialise­d scent secretions, but urine and dung require no extra energy costs. Dung is larger and therefore easier to find, but the longevity of dung versus urine is species-specific.’’

Plenty of other animals, both plant- and meat-eaters, use such communal pooping grounds – and identifyin­g the telltale compounds in their dung could open a new door to studying them as well as to creating new tools for land management and species conservati­on.

‘‘For example, encouragin­g animals to cross a corridor to increase genetic flow, or discouragi­ng animals from dangerous areas such as human conflict zones,’’ Marneweck said, noting a few of the possibilit­ies. ‘‘There are even captive applicatio­ns such as aiding breeding programmes by providing olfactory stimuli.’’ – Los Angeles Times

 ??  ?? Rhinoceros­es leave very specific ‘‘posts’’ about themselves at communal defecation sites, according to new research.
Rhinoceros­es leave very specific ‘‘posts’’ about themselves at communal defecation sites, according to new research.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from New Zealand