Otago Daily Times

Ideology trumps truth

Researcher­s have found voters have a high tolerance of politician­s who lie, even those who are caught doing it. Melissa Healy, of the Los Angeles Times, reports.

-

IN a modern democracy, peddling conspiraci­es for political advantage is perhaps not so different from seeding an epidemic.

If a virus is to gain a foothold with the electorate, it will need a population of likely believers (‘‘susceptibl­es’’ in public health speak), a germ nimble enough to infect new hosts easily (an irresistib­le tall tale), and an eager ‘‘Amen choir’’ (also known as ‘‘supersprea­ders’’).

Unleashed on the body politic, a falsehood may spread across the social networks that supply us with informatio­n. Facebook is a doorknob slathered in germs, Twitter a sneezing coworker, and Instagram a child returning home after a day at school, ensuring the exposure of all.

But if lies, conspiraci­es and fake news are really like germs, you might think that factchecki­ng is the cure, and truth an effective antidote.

If only it were that easy.

New research offers fresh insights into the stubborn role of ideology in maintainin­g support for those who peddle falsehoods, and the limited power of factchecki­ng to change voters’ minds. Even in the face of immediate and authoritat­ive correction­s, we humans don’t budge easily, or for long, from establishe­d opinions about politics, politician­s and the coverage they receive.

And some of us — in particular, those who endorse conservati­ve positions — are quicker to believe assertions that warn of grim consequenc­es or of sinister forces at work.

The findings of three new studies suggest factchecke­rs had better be persistent, and that their expectatio­ns of changing people’s minds had better be modest.

But the research also suggests that if factchecke­rs want the truth to matter, they should not be shy about touting the value of their services.

Arguably, the need for factchecki­ng has never been greater. The Washington Post’s ‘‘Fact Checker’’, which maintains a running tally of US President Donald Trump’s false statements, has counted 6420 false or misleading statements from the president through to October 30, including more than 4400 this year. A Fact Checker poll released last week found that more than six in 10 Americans believe factchecke­rs when they conclude Trump has made a false claim — meaning more than onethird of them do not.

Is credulity, and a vague mistrust of factchecke­rs, unique to Americans, or is it a broader attribute of humans? It may be a bit of both.

In a study published last Wednesday and conducted with a sample of 370 Australian­s, researcher­s found the veracity of a political candidate’s claims does matter to voters — sometimes. When Australian subjects were shown an array of politician­s’ false statements corrected by factchecki­ng, they reduced their belief of those assertions. When they were shown factchecke­d true statements, whether attributed to a politician on the right or one on the left, their belief in the assertions increased as well.

This factchecki­ng changed subjects’ views about which politician­s they supported, but only slightly — only when false statements outnumbere­d true statements by a ratio of four to one. When false statements and true statements were attributed to a candidate in equal numbers — four falsehoods in balance with four true statements — Australian subjects didn’t change their opinions at all.

Study coauthor Adam J. Berinksy, a political scientist at MIT, said he considered those results a bit less depressing than what he found when he tried the same experiment on American subjects. When the authors presented factchecke­d assertions from Trump and former Democratic presidenti­al candidate Bernie Sanders to Americans, ‘‘the magnitude of the overall effect was minute’’, even when false statements outweighed true ones by the same fourtoone margin.

Those results, which are not yet published, suggest that, although both Americans and Australian­s are capable of distinguis­hing fact from fiction (with help from factchecke­rs), they are loath to alter their overall view of their favoured candidate accordingl­y.

‘‘They seem to be saying, ‘He may be a liar, but he’s my liar’,’’ Berinsky said.

Also ‘‘slightly depressing’’, he added, was the short shelf life of a factcheck: A week after subjects in both countries saw politician­s’ assertions corrected for truthfulne­ss, they had forgotten virtually all of what they had learned.

But Berinsky said he took heart in Australian­s’ willingnes­s to adjust their assessment­s of lying politician­s even a little bit.

‘‘I mainly study US politics and am used to a world in which factchecki­ng doesn’t work very well, where people are really stuck in their lane and politician­s are seemingly immune to any kind of facts,’’ he said. ‘‘It’s good to know there are countries in which this still can work.’’

The findings echoed those of a report published earlier this month in Plos One, which demonstrat­ed that the inclusion of factchecki­ng in an experiment­al news feed made subjects hungrier and more confident news consumers. It also made them more inclined to trust ‘‘mainstream media outlets’’.

But there was a hitch: In addition to being very small, subjects’ shifts in attitude became evident only when their news feeds included an occasional ‘‘defence of journalism’’ article. Usually, these were opinion pieces that countered attacks on the profession.

‘‘Without defence of journalism, factchecki­ng had no effect on any of these outcomes,’’ Raymond J. Pingree, a professor of mass communicat­ions at Louisiana State University, and his coauthor concluded.

Selfidenti­fied Republican­s in the study started out lower than Democrats in their trust of mainstream media, their confidence in their own ability to decide what is true in politics, and their intention to use a mainstream news portal in the future. But after a week of plying them with specialise­d news feeds, Pingree’s team found that people across the political spectrum responded well to the combinatio­n of factchecki­ng and defenceofj­ournalism pieces.

If you are starting to see a light at the end of the partisan tunnel, however, consider a third study published last week. It tested the idea that people are more inclined to believe unproven conspiracy

theories when their party is out of power, a notion sometimes called the ‘‘conspiracy belief is for losers’’ hypothesis.

The study was led by UCLA anthropolo­gist Daniel Fessler, who found that people whose political stances aligned them with American conservati­sm were far more likely than liberals to embrace falsehoods that warned of grim consequenc­es.

Americans who hew to more progressiv­e political stances were certainly credulous as well, the UCLA team found. But they were no more likely to believe a scary falsehood — say, that a drunken airline passenger could pry open a plane’s door in midair — than they were to buy into the far less terrifying myth that you can burn more calories by exercising on an empty stomach.

But were these inclinatio­ns real and enduring, or could they be explained by the fact that, when the experiment was run in October 2015 and September 2016, conservati­ves had been out of the White House for several years?

Fessler and Theodore Samore, a graduate student in UCLA’s anthropolo­gy department, repeated the experiment in 2016, after Donald Trump had won the presidenti­al election, and in 2017, after Georgia Democrat Doug Jones beat Republican Roy Moore in a special election for a Senate seat. After Trump’s triumph, the researcher­s reasoned, conservati­ves should feel empowered and confident. After Jones’ victory, they presumed, liberals would probably feel hopeful once more.

But their original findings did not change: As they moved further right on the ideologica­l spectrum, people were consistent­ly more likely to believe frightenin­g false claims, and found them more credible than emotionall­y neutral falsehoods. The results were published earlier this month in Plos One.

‘‘It seems there’s just a fundamenta­l difference in how credulous people are about hazards as a function of their orientatio­n,’’ Fessler said.

‘‘How positively people feel about their party’s future doesn’t matter.’’

That dynamic has worrisome implicatio­ns: When believers of ominous warnings succeed at the polls, ‘‘they have the megaphone that power brings’’, Fessler said. ‘‘And they use that — whether cynically or genuinely I can’t tell — to issue additional proclamati­ons of danger.’’

This, he said, had been Trump’s stock in trade — foreign powers are taking advantage of the United States, dangerous hordes are storming the borders, and we need to build a wall to keep wouldbe invaders at bay.

‘‘That cycle is very difficult to break,’’ Fessler said.

What’s more, warning people who are inclined to believe that kind of narrative that they are being lied to seems more likely to reinforce the conspiracy theory than to induce a change of heart.

‘‘I do worry,’’ he said. — TNS

 ??  ??
 ?? PHOTO: GETTY IMAGES ?? Believe me . . . The Washington Post’s ‘‘Fact Checker’’ has counted 6420 false or misleading statements made by US President Donald Trump up until October 30.
PHOTO: GETTY IMAGES Believe me . . . The Washington Post’s ‘‘Fact Checker’’ has counted 6420 false or misleading statements made by US President Donald Trump up until October 30.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from New Zealand