Taranaki Daily News

LANGUAGE MATTERS Pseudo -words

- Andreea Calude Senior lecturer in Linguistic­s at Waikato University

Kids love to play around with language as they learn it. They delight in testing out new words too. For instance, my kids talk about the worser things in life, knowing full well that worser is not really a word in English.

This kind of play is part of the learning process. Children’s authors are also known to make up interestin­g words, like Roald Dahl’s frothbungl­ing, describing something ridiculous, or Dr Seuss’ miffmuffer­ed moof, which refers to the material from which the Once-ler makes his clothes in The Lorax.

Quite apart from eliciting a good laugh and the opportunit­y for an in-joke among children, nonwords can be very useful for adults, too. Psychologi­sts sometimes use them to test how individual­s process and store language knowledge. Language teachers also use them, in language proficienc­y tests.

For practical reasons, language vocabulary tests may involve asking learners if they know a word, and simply trusting them to tell the truth. In order to ensure that learners do not optimistic­ally exaggerate their vocabulary knowledge, non-words which look like the real thing are snuck into the lists. Every time such a non-word is selected as known, points are deducted from the overall score.

In fact, so useful are these non-words that researcher­s have a jargon term reserved just for them: pseudoword­s. Given how pseudoword­s are used, we really want to make them appear very much like the real deal.

It would be useless to include sdkjhakfhk­ajdf as a word in a list testing English learners, because anyone who knows anything about English will immediatel­y smell a rat. But coming up with good pseudoword­s is no mean feat. Not everyone has children lying around the place actively engaged in wordplay all day long.

And for those who don’t, a faster way to get pseudoword­s is to program a computer to generate them. Computers can do this by splitting existing words into parts and then recombinin­g these in new ways.

A few years ago, one of my former PhD students, Jemma Ko¨ nig, came up with a neat algorithm for generating pseudoword­s. Sometimes this worked really well and we got gems like novelines, wordinaril­y, unimagine and apartmenta­lize, leading us to wonder why English does not have these as real words; they seemed to have such potential! But sometimes, the process did not work quite as well: istye, thwiped or prirr.

The trouble is that, while they can generate pseudoword­s, computers cannot (currently) rate how good their made-up, fake words are.

Jemma and I sifted through the output to come up with ways of rating the pseudoword­s’ ability to pass as English words. This wasn’t easy either. As speakers of English, we could immediatel­y pick out the good pseudoword­s from the not-so-good ones, but in order to figure out how our minds were doing this, we had to reverse engineer the process, and things got tricky.

All this goes to show just how remarkable our minds are in their ability to capture language patterns implicitly, and instantly recognise items which do not match these. We know so much about language and comparativ­ely still so little about how we know this, or what this exact knowledge is.

As it turns out, wordinaril­y speaking, far from being frothbungl­ing, pseudoword­s may have more to teach us, in their simplicit way, than we have ever previously unimagined.

 ??  ?? Children’s authors are known to make up interestin­g words, like Dr Seuss’ miffmuffer­ed moof.
Children’s authors are known to make up interestin­g words, like Dr Seuss’ miffmuffer­ed moof.
 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from New Zealand