Waikato Times

Rumours spread fast, false or true

- CASS R SUNSTEIN Opinion

Did you hear? Taylor Swift is doing a new album, consisting of her favorite Katy Perry songs – and despite their lengthy feud, Perry herself will be performing on the album!

OK, that’s not true. But a new study finds that by every measure, false rumors are more likely to spread than true ones. For those who believe in the marketplac­e of ideas and democratic self-government, that’s a big problem, raising an obvious question: What, if anything, are we going to do about it?

The study, conducted by Soroush Vosoughi, Deb Roy and Sinan Aral of the Massachuse­tts Institute of Technology, was based on a massive data set, consisting of all fact-checked rumor ‘‘cascades’’ that spread on Twitter from 2006 to 2017. All in all, there were about 126,000 such cascades, spread by about three million people more than 4.5 million times.

To test whether truth was stronger than falsehood, the researcher­s looked at rumours that had been fact-checked by six independen­t organisati­ons (Snopes.com, PolitiFact.com, FactCheck.org, TruthOrFic­tion.com, Hoax-Slayer.com and UrbanLegen­dsOnline.com). The organisati­ons reached the same conclusion about these rumors at least 95 per cent of the time. The central questions: Did falsehoods get retweeted more often? Were they more likely to go viral?

The answers were clear: Yes and yes. Using careful statistica­l tests, Vosoughi and his co-authors find that ‘‘falsehood diffused significan­tly farther, faster, deeper, and more broadly than the truth in all categories of informatio­n.’’

For example, falsehoods reached 1500 people six times more rapidly than truth. And while false statements about business, science and entertainm­ent did better than true ones, the biggest difference was in the domain of politics.

Importantl­y, the researcher­s found that falsehoods do not spread only or mostly because of the actions of ‘‘bots.’’ Vosoughi and his co-authors re-ran their study while using a bot-detection algorithm to identify and remove all bots – and they found that all of their main conclusion­s held. Human beings, it seems, are far more likely to spread falsehood than truth.

The researcher­s speculate that one reason may be novelty. Enlisting a variety of metrics to test whether tweets convey new informatio­n, they find that ‘‘false rumours were measurably more novel than true rumours’’. It’s reasonable to hypothesis­e that novel informatio­n is more likely to spread, and that hypothesis may help to explain the popularity of falsehoods.

Psychologi­sts have also found that rumours are more likely to spread if they produce identifiab­le emotions, such as disgust. Vosoughi and his coauthors compared the emotional content of replies to true and false rumours. They found that truth produced greater sadness, trust and anticipati­on – while falsehoods produced greater surprise and disgust.

These are striking and important findings, but it’s possible to raise some questions. Vosoughi and his colleagues do not really show that falsehoods are more likely to spread than truth. More precisely, they find that within the category of popular rumours tested by independen­t fact-finding bodies, the false ones are especially likely to spread.

That’s an important distinctio­n, because plenty of falsehoods don’t spread. If I tweeted that the Michigan Law Review is now publishing its 112th volume, that the population of Germany is 85 million, that Carl Yastrzemsk­i won baseball’s Triple Crown in 1969, or that Section 553 of the Administra­tive Procedure Act governs adjudicati­on, people wouldn’t be all that interested, even though every one of these statements is false.

At the same time, lots of true statements get tons of attention. Consider those involving Robert Mueller’s investigat­ion, or Stormy Daniels’ lawsuit against Donald Trump, or the latest statements and actions of North Korean leader Kim Jong Un.

In short, independen­t fact-checkers investigat­e only a very small subset of both false and true statements. Of that subset, the false ones may be especially provocativ­e and interestin­g, above all in the political domain.

Anticipati­ng this objection, Vosoughi and his co-authors also had their students study a sample of rumour cascades that had not been verified by fact-finding organisati­ons. Their central conclusion held: Rumours found to be false spread more quickly than rumours found to be true. But that study could not possibly explore the full universe of true and false rumours, including the many false rumours that are deadly dull and get little or no attention.

Even with these qualificat­ions, the new research is highly significan­t, because it shows that demonstrab­ly false rumours receive a great deal of attention on social media. But what’s the best response?

Vosoughi and his co-authors conclude that ‘‘misinforma­tioncontai­nment policies’’ should include ‘‘behavioura­l interventi­ons, like labelling and incentives to dissuade the spread of misinforma­tion’’. That might be right, but it’s pretty vague. Suppose that those who run Twitter, Facebook and other social media platforms were determined to reduce the spread of demonstrab­ly false statements, at least when those statements are highly likely to cause serious harm.

It would be useful, if only as a thought experiment, to specify possible responses. Social media platforms could rely on the marketplac­e of ideas – and do nothing. They could get pretty aggressive – and immediatel­y delete the false statements. They could offer correction­s, red flags or vivid warnings. One or another approach would make sense in imaginable contexts. In the coming years, the question deserves sustained attention, with particular focus on what current platforms are, and aren’t, doing right now.

 ?? PHOTOS: GETTY IMAGES ?? Did you hear the rumours about Katy Perry and Taylor Swift?
PHOTOS: GETTY IMAGES Did you hear the rumours about Katy Perry and Taylor Swift?
 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from New Zealand