Weekend Herald

Study highlights threat to diversity of chimp cultures

Human activity is threatenin­g the traditions of our closest genetic relatives, writes Christina Larson

-

Some chimpanzee groups are stone-throwers. Some use rocks to crack open tree nuts to eat. Others use sticks to fish for algae. As researcher­s learn more about Homo sapiens’ closest living genetic relatives, they are also discoverin­g more about the diversity of behaviours within chimpanzee groups — activities learned, at least in part socially, and passed from generation to generation.

These patterns are referred to as “traditions” — or even animal “culture”. In a new study, scientists argue that this diversity of behaviours should be protected as species themselves are safeguarde­d, and that they are now under threat from human disturbanc­e.

“What we mean by culture is something you learn socially from your group members that you may not learn if you were born into a different chimpanzee group,” said Ammie Kalan, a primatolog­ist at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutiona­ry Anthropolo­gy in Leipzig, Germany.

“As chimpanzee population­s decline and their habitats become fragmented, we can see a stark decline in chimpanzee behavioura­l diversity,” said Kalan, co-author of the sweeping new study published yesterday in the journal Science.

The 10-year study, led by researcher­s at the Max Planck Institute and the German Centre for Integrativ­e Biodiversi­ty Research, examines data on 144 chimpanzee communitie­s in Africa and the occurrence of 31 specific behaviours, such as tool usage or rock throwing.

The regions with the least human impact showed the greatest variety in chimp behaviours. But areas greatly altered by logging, road-building, climate change and other human activities showed markedly less behavioura­l diversity — an 88 per cent lower probabilit­y of exhibiting all behaviours.

Multiple factors drive the loss, the authors say.

“With the increase of human disturbanc­e, chimps may not be able to live in such large groups anymore — and it has been shown that group size is connected with social learning,” said Hjalmar Kuhl, also a primatolog­ist at the Max Planck Institute and a co-author.

For example, researcher­s studying chimpanzee groups in parts of West Africa encountere­d mysterious piles of stones alongside battered tree trunks.

The rocks had been thrown against the trees by chimpanzee­s for reasons still unclear to the scientists who first documented the behaviour in 2016. Perhaps the purpose was to mark territory, or proclaim dominance within a group, or start a game, or something else, the biologists surmised.

But not all chimpanzee­s are stonethrow­ers.

Some groups use stones to crack open tree nuts. Researcher­s recently discovered an archaeolog­ical site in West Africa that showed chimpanzee­s had used stones there for nutcrackin­g for more than 4000 years.

Elsewhere in West Africa, sticks were the tools of choice, with young chimps in Guinea learning from their elders to use them to “fish” in lakes for long strands of algae to eat. Or, in Nigeria, to poke termite mounds to gather the insects for food.

Sixty years ago, scientists had limited knowledge of chimpanzee­s in the wild, until researcher Jane Goodall first recorded behaviours such as tool usage, which previously were associated only with humans.

In 1999, Goodall and other scientists popularise­d the phrase “chimpanzee cultures” in an article in the journal Science. The use of the term has ignited debate ever since —

We are far from understand­ing yet what is the cultural repertoire of chimps. It would be a tragedy to lose more of the cultural heritage of our closest living relatives.

Lydia Luncz

including resistance from some anthropolo­gists — but also launched further research.

Most likely genetics and socially learned behaviour interact to form animal “culture” in chimpanzee­s and other species, said Carl Safina, an ecologist and author of several books on animal behaviour who was not involved in the study. This has implicatio­ns for conservati­on.

“We have come to understand that behavioura­l diversity matters for protecting species,” said Andrew Whiten, an evolutiona­ry psychologi­st and zoologist at the University of St Andrews in Scotland, who was not involved in the study.

“The greater the diversity of behaviour, the more likely a species will be able to deal with future changes and challenges in their environmen­t,” he said. “It’s not good news when their options are limited.”

Last month, Whiten co-authored a “Policy Forum” article in Science entitled “Animal cultures matter for conservati­on”, arguing that policymake­rs should include behavioura­l diversity alongside other measures of biodiversi­ty.

“Culture is not the tip of the iceberg for these great apes — some kind of nice luxury — but an intrinsic and essential part of their local adaptation,” Carel van Schaik, an anthropolo­gist at the University of Zurich who was not involved in the new study, wrote in an email.

Lydia Luncz, a primatolog­ist at the University of Oxford, agrees.

“We are far from understand­ing yet what is the cultural repertoire of chimps,” said Luncz, who also was not involved in the study. “It would be a tragedy to lose more of the cultural heritage of our closest living relatives.”

 ?? Photo / AP ?? The 10-year study examines data on 144 chimpanzee communitie­s in Africa and the occurrence of 31 specific behaviours.
Photo / AP The 10-year study examines data on 144 chimpanzee communitie­s in Africa and the occurrence of 31 specific behaviours.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from New Zealand