Daily Maverick

A good lesson in teaching maths

What we’ve learnt from cognitive science should be put to use in how the subject is taught. By

- Colin Foster DM First published by The Conversati­on. Colin Foster is a reader in mathematic­s education at Loughborou­gh University in England.

School mathematic­s teaching is stuck in the past. An adult revisiting the school they attended as a child would see only superficia­l changes from what they experience­d themselves.

Yes, in some schools they might see a room full of electronic tablets, or the teacher using a touch-sensitive, interactiv­e whiteboard. But if we zoom in on the details – the tasks that students are actually being given to help them make sense of the subject – things have hardly changed at all.

We’ve learnt a huge amount in recent years about cognitive science – how our brains work and how people learn most effectivel­y. This understand­ing has the potential to revolution­ise what teachers do in classrooms. But the design of mathematic­s teaching materials such as textbooks has benefited very little from this knowledge.

Some of this knowledge is counterint­uitive, and therefore unlikely to be applied unless done so deliberate­ly. What learners prefer to experience and what teachers think is likely to be most effective are often not what will help the most.

For example, cognitive science tells us that practising similar kinds of tasks all together generally leads to less effective learning than mixing up tasks that require different approaches.

In mathematic­s, practising similar tasks together could be a page of questions, each of which requires addition of fractions. Mixing things up might involve bringing together fractions, probabilit­y and equations in immediate succession.

Learners make more mistakes when doing mixed exercises and are likely to feel frustrated by this. Grouping similar tasks together is therefore likely to be much easier for the teacher to manage. But the mixed exercises give the learner important practice in deciding what method they need to use for each question. This means that more knowledge is retained afterwards, making it what is known as a “desirable difficulty”.

Cognitive science applied

We are just now beginning to apply findings like this from cognitive science to design better teaching materials and support teachers in using them. Focusing on school mathematic­s makes sense because mathematic­s is a compulsory subject that many people find difficult to learn.

Typically, school teaching materials are chosen by gut reactions. A head of department looks at a new textbook scheme and, based on their experience, chooses whatever seems best to them. What else can they be expected to do? But even the best materials on offer are generally not designed with cognitive science principles such as “desirable difficulti­es” in mind.

My colleagues and I have been researchin­g educationa­l design that applies principles from cognitive science to mathematic­s teaching and are developing materials for schools. These materials are not designed to look easy, but to include “desirable difficulti­es”.

They are not divided into individual lessons, because this pushes the teacher towards moving on when the clock says so, regardless of student needs.

Being responsive to students’ developing understand­ing and difficulti­es requires materials designed according to the size of the ideas, rather than what will fit convenient­ly on a double-page spread of a textbook or into a 40-minute class period.

Switching things up

Taking an approach led by cognitive science also means changing how mathematic­al concepts are explained.

For instance, diagrams have always been a prominent feature of mathematic­s teaching, but often they are used haphazardl­y, based on the teacher’s personal preference. In textbooks they are highly restricted because of space constraint­s.

Often, similar-looking diagrams are used in different topics and for very different purposes, leading to confusion. For example, three circles connected (as shown left) can indicate partitioni­ng into a sum – the “partwhole model” – or a product of prime factors. These involve two very different operations, but they are frequently represente­d by the same diagram.

Using the same kind of diagram to represent conflictin­g operations (addition and multiplica­tion) leads to learners muddling them up and becoming confused.

The “coherence principle” from cognitive science means avoiding diagrams where their drawbacks outweigh their benefits, and using diagrams and animations in a purposeful, consistent way across topics.

For example, number lines can be introduced at a young age and incorporat­ed across many topic areas to bring coherence to students’ developing understand­ing of numbers. Number lines can be used to solve equations and also to represent probabilit­ies, for instance.

Unlike with the circle diagrams, the uses of number lines (shown left) do not conflict but reinforce each other.

In each case, positions on the number line represent numbers, from zero on the left, increasing to the right.

There are disturbing inequaliti­es in the learning of mathematic­s, with students from poorer background­s underachie­ving relative to their wealthier peers. There is also a huge gender participat­ion gap in maths, in matric and beyond, which is taken by far more boys than girls.

Socioecono­mically advantaged families have always been able to buy their children out of difficulti­es by using private tutors, but less privileged families cannot. Better quality teaching materials, based on insights from cognitive science, mitigate the impact for students who have traditiona­lly been disadvanta­ged by gender, race or financial background in the learning of mathematic­s.

 ?? ??
 ?? Diagrams: Colin Foster ??
Diagrams: Colin Foster
 ?? Photo: Freepik ??
Photo: Freepik

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from South Africa