The Independent on Saturday

Gaming is not an addiction

Comparison to illegal drugs and alcohol is misguided

- CHRISTOPHE­R FERGUSON and PATRICK MARKEY

IT’S CERTAINLY common to hear parents complain that their children are “addicted” to video games. Some researcher­s even claim these games are comparable to illegal drugs in terms of their influence on the brain – that they are “digital heroin” or “digital pharmakeia”.

The American Psychiatri­c Associatio­n has identified internet gaming disorder as a possible psychiatri­c illness, and the WHO has proposed including “gaming disorder” in its catalogue of mental diseases, along with drug and alcohol addiction.

This is all terribly misguided. Playing video games is not addictive in any meaningful sense. It is normal behaviour that, while perhaps in many cases a waste of time, is not damaging or disruptive of lives in the way drug or alcohol use can be.

Let’s start with the neuroscien­tific analogy: that the areas in the brain associated with the pleasures of drug use are the same as those associated with the pleasures of playing video games.

This is true but not illuminati­ng. These areas of the brain – those that produce and respond to the neurotrans­mitter dopamine – are involved in just about any pleasurabl­e activity: having sex, enjoying a nice conversati­on, eating good food, reading a book, using methamphet­amines. The amount of dopamine involved in these activities, however, differs widely. Playing a video game or watching an amusing video on the internet causes roughly about as much dopamine to be released in your brain as eating a slice of pizza.

By contrast, using a drug like methamphet­amine can cause a level of dopamine release 10 times that or more. On its own, the fact that a pleasurabl­e activity involves dopamine release tells us nothing else about it.

A large-scale study of internet-based games recently published in the American Journal of Psychiatry bears out our scepticism about this “addiction.” Using the American Psychiatri­c Associatio­n’s own metrics for ascertaini­ng psychiatri­c disorder, the study’s researcher­s found that, at most, 1% of video game players might exhibit characteri­stics of an addiction and the games were significan­tly less addictive than, say, gambling.

Addiction

More damning, the study found that almost none of those classified as being possibly addicted to video games experience­d negative outcomes from this addiction. That is, the mental, physical and social health of these potential “addicts” was not different from that of individual­s who were not “addicted” to video games. This suggests that the diagnosis of addiction doesn’t make much sense to begin with.

The biggest difference between addicts and non-addicts the study found, was that the addicts played more video games. This diagnosis verges on mere tautology.

The risk here, of course, is that by treating the immoderate playing of video games as an addiction, we are pathologis­ing relatively normal behaviour. Consider a common diagnostic question used to help identify addiction, such as “I always use X to relax after a bad day.” Well, if X is methamphet­amine, that’s a worrisome choice, one that presumably indicates addiction. But if X is playing video games, how is that different from unwinding after work by knitting, watching sport or playing bridge?

We don’t deny that new technologi­es come with some perils. We understand the nostalgia for the halcyon days of, say, the 1950s, when people were not yet bound to their personal technology and were free to enjoy the simpler pleasures of life, like climbing trees – and getting polio and having to wait in a queue at the bank to check your account balance.

We and our children are “addicted” to new technologi­es because they improve our lives or are simply pleasurabl­e.

Evidence for addiction to video games is virtually non-existent. That’s not to say we and our children don’t need to balance technology use with exercise, proper sleep and the responsibi­lities we have in real life. But indulging in panic about technology or nostalgia for a better past that never really existed does us no good. – New York Times

Christophe­r J Ferguson, a professor of psychology at Stetson University, and Patrick Markey, a professor of psychology at Villanova, are the authors of Moral Combat: Why the War on Video Games is Wrong.

 ??  ?? GAME ON: Two experts say playing video games is normal behaviour. It may be a waste of time but it is not damaging or disruptive to lives in the way drug or alcohol use can be.
GAME ON: Two experts say playing video games is normal behaviour. It may be a waste of time but it is not damaging or disruptive to lives in the way drug or alcohol use can be.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from South Africa