Gulf News

How kids learn prejudice in the US

Past research from psychology suggests that a ‘Trump effect’ on children’s attitudes is very likely real

-

ecently, my two-and-a-half-year-old daughter asked me about Republican presidenti­al nominee Donald Trump’s video everyone seemed to be talking about. Like many parents, I had made the mistake of assuming we were still in the soft and squishy baby days when she wasn’t listening. But now she is listening.

I told her that a man who would like to be president said some mean things that hurt a lot of people’s feelings. My daughter started to cry; like many children, she is sensitive. I hugged her and assured her that everything would be OK. But as a psychology professor who studies the developmen­t of social attitudes, I had to ask myself, will it really be OK?

At the second presidenti­al debate, Democratic presidenti­al nominee Hillary Clinton talked about the “Trump effect”, a rise — anecdotall­y, at least — in bullying in schools. Are children adopting the negative attitudes that Republican presidenti­al nominee Trump’s campaign has too often promoted? Do they feel a newfound sense of permissibi­lity in mocking people, as Trump has, based on their race, their religion, their gender or their disability?

It will take time for research on education to answer those questions fully. But past research from psychology suggests that a “Trump effect” on children’s attitudes is very likely real.

Children are cultural sponges: They absorb the mores that surround them — how to dress, what to eat, what to say. This is a good thing, all in all, since a major function of childhood is figuring out how to be a proficient adult in a particular society. This means picking up on social norms. Unfortunat­ely, this includes learning your society’s explicit and implicit views of the status and worth of different social groups.

Developmen­tal psychology research has shown that by the time they start kindergart­en, children begin to show many of the same implicit racial attitudes that adults in our culture hold. Children have already learnt to associate some groups with higher status, or more positive value, than others.

Subcultura­l cues

An associatio­n between status and group membership can be learnt surprising­ly quickly. Psychologi­sts Kristin Shutts, Kristina R. Olson and Suzanne R. Horwitz recently demonstrat­ed that in just a few minutes of exposure in a laboratory setting to informatio­n about fictional groups with differing socioecono­mic status, children picked up on which groups were wealthier — and indicated that they liked those people better.

Gender attitudes, too, form early and can be influenced by subtle cultural cues. For instance, in experiment­s in preschool and elementary school classrooms, teachers were instructed to make a bigger deal of gender than they typically would. They treated girls and boys equally positively, but they highlighte­d that the two genders were different — for instance, boys and girls hung their art on different walls, and children were labelled often as being “boys” and “girls.” Note that the children were not taught anything explicit about gender stereotype­s.

Yet after a few weeks, they started to endorse broader stereotype­s about gender. For example, they became more likely to think that boys, but not girls, should become scientists.

It seems that merely marking a category — suggesting to children that it matters — led them to pick up on cultural stereotype­s. It is also important to consider that negative informatio­n is particular­ly compelling to children. For example, my colleagues and I have found that when children learn about people committing anti-social actions, they remember those actions in greater detail than they do with comparable positive actions. Talking about entire groups of people as being threatenin­g or dangerous, as Trump has done, is precisely the kind of language that children are likely to internalis­e.

Now, all hope is not lost. America has made progress on many issues of social bias, and younger generation­s tend to be more open-minded and tolerant of different groups than older generation­s are. Research by psychologi­sts Melissa Ferguson, Thomas Mann and Jeremy Cone shows that with sufficient countervai­ling positive informatio­n, even initially negative implicit attitudes about people can be unlearned. But we need to remember that what’s at stake in a Trump presidency is not just his policy choices, his approach to diplomacy and his having a finger on the nuclear trigger. Also at stake are the attitudes Trump’s discourse would transmit to a generation of children.

Electing Clinton, in addition to offering a wholly different set of policy positions, will also help teach children that America is a place where little girls can grow up to be scientists, and maybe even president.

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Arab Emirates