Gulf News

How our brain clickbaits us

There is a peculiar reason why we aren’t curious about the things we want to be curious about

-

You can learn anything on the internet, so why do I so often learn things I don’t want to know? When I’m surfing the web I want to be drawn in by articles on Europe’s political history or the nature of quasars, but I end up reading trivia like a menu from Alcatraz prison. Why am I not curious about the things I want to be curious about?

Curiosity feels like it’s outside your control, and trying to direct it sounds as ill conceived as forcing yourself to find a joke funny. But if you understand what prompts curiosity, you may be able to channel it a little better.

Across evolutiona­ry time, curious animals were more likely to survive because they learnt about their environmen­ts; a forager that occasional­ly skipped a reliable feeding ground to explore might find an even better place to eat.

Humans, too, will forgo a known payoff to investigat­e the unknown. In one experiment, subjects were asked to choose one of four photos, each carrying some chance of paying a cash prize. Photos repeated, so subjects learnt to pick the best-paying, but when a novel photo popped up, they chose it more often than the odds dictated they should. This preference for novelty is, of course, the reason manufactur­ers periodical­ly tweak product packaging and advertisin­g.

But it’s good to know about your environmen­t even if it doesn’t promise a reward right now; knowledge may be useless today, but vital next week. Therefore, evolution has left us with a brain that can reward itself; satisfying curiosity feels pleasurabl­e, so you explore the environmen­t even when you don’t expect any concrete payoff. Infants prefer to look at novel pictures compared with familiar ones. Preschoole­rs play longer with a mechanical toy if it’s difficult to deduce how it works.

What’s more, curiosity doesn’t just ensure new opportunit­ies for learning, it enhances learning itself. In a recent experiment, subjects read trivia questions and rated how curious each made them feel. Later, they saw the questions again, each followed by a photograph of a face, and judged whether that person looked like he or she would know the answer. In a surprise final memory test for the faces, subjects better remembered those appearing after trivia questions that made them curious. Curiosity causes a brain state that amplifies learning.

This function of curiosity — to heighten memory — is the key to understand­ing why we’re curious about some things and not others. We feel most curious when exploratio­n will yield the most learning.

Suppose I asked you: “What’s the most common type of star in the Milky Way?” You’ll obviously feel no curiosity if you already know the answer. But you’ll also feel little interest if you know nothing about stars; if you learnt the answer, you couldn’t connect it to other knowledge, so it would seem nearly meaningles­s, an isolated factoid. We’re maximally curious when we sense that the environmen­t offers new informatio­n in the right proportion to complement what we already know.

Note that your brain calculates what you might learn in the short term — your long-term interests aren’t a factor. That’s why a cardiac surgeon who is passionate about her job will neverthele­ss find a conference presentati­on on the subject boring if her brain decides that the talk won’t add to her knowledge. Conversely, when a friend persuades her to watch a documentar­y on type fonts, her brain may calculate that this will be a rich source of informatio­n — and she finds herself fascinated.

Long-term interests

It’s that disconnect between long and short-term interests that makes frothy articles so frustratin­g. The feeling of curiosity promised you’d learn something and, admittedly, you did — now you know French citizens’ favourite macaron flavour — but you’re disappoint­ed because your new knowledge doesn’t contribute to your long-term interests. You’ve been clickbaite­d by your own brain.

If following curiosity results in disappoint­ment, maybe it shouldn’t be allowed to take the lead. Why not just search for topics you truly want to learn about? That sounds logical, but a search for “quasars” will yield thousands of hits and no way of knowing which offers the just-right match to your current knowledge that will maintain your curiosity. You’ll probably end up like the surgeon at the boring conference talk.

If you wish for more serious reading when you surf the web, the opportunis­tic approach is actually fine. You just need to frequent better foraging grounds.

Many websites that snare your time feature scores of stories on the front page, banking that one will strike each reader’s sweet spot of knowledge. So visit websites that use the same strategy but offer richer content.

And pay more attention to bylines. Curiosity arises from the right balance of the familiar and the novel. Naturally, writers vary in what they assume their audience already knows and wants to know; when you find an author who tends to have your number, stick with her.

Einstein famously advised a young student to “never lose a holy curiosity”. Given our evolutiona­ry history, there’s little danger any of us will. The challenge is changing its focus from the momentary to something more enduring. ■ Daniel T. Willingham is a leading psychologi­st at the University of Virginia, where he is a professor in the Department of Psychology. He is a pioneering name in cognitive psychology and neuroscien­ce.

 ?? Jose Luis Barros/©Gulf News ??
Jose Luis Barros/©Gulf News

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Arab Emirates