The National - News

Men and women are not really worlds apart

- JUSTIN THOMAS

Challengin­g gender inequality is a choice. Since 1911, March 8 has been Internatio­nal Women’s Day. This year’s theme centres on challengin­g and calling out gender bias and inequality wherever it rears its ugly, sometimes unconsciou­s, head. The occasion is a celebratio­n of women’s achievemen­ts in the social, economic, cultural and political spheres. The day is being promoted worldwide with the social media hashtag #ChooseToCh­allenge.

Beyond the balloons, hashtags and rubber wristbands, this is a global call for gender parity. Internatio­nal Women’s Day, first and foremost, is about the eradicatio­n of gender inequality, and ensuring equal access to resources and opportunit­ies.

The first step is an awareness that problemati­c gender difference­s exist. The social sciences, and psychology, in particular, have a long history of uncovering such difference­s. It is almost a cliche that undergradu­ate students do research comparing some attribute or another across genders – for example, men versus women on self-esteem. This isn’t always useful or interestin­g. It is just easy to do.

Knowing that there is a gender difference is not as useful as knowing why the discrepanc­y exists. For example, in many nations, women are diagnosed with clinical depression at much higher rates than men. In the community psychiatri­c survey in Al Ain, for instance, depressed women outnumbere­d depressed men almost 4 to 1. The authors of this study, published in Social Psychiatry and Psychiatri­c Epidemiolo­gy in 2001, suggested that gender difference was related to the reluctance of men to report depressive symptoms.

But why would men be less likely to report the symptoms of depression? Here, we get to the heart of the matter. The late Susan Nolen-Hoeksema headed the Yale Depression and Cognition Programme in the US. For her, gender difference­s in depression started in childhood. She argued that young boys are typically discourage­d from “sissy” displays of emotion, whereas young girls are indulged. Girls and boys are also bombarded with ill-founded tropes, such as the idea that “women are naturally emotional”. For Nolen-Hoeksema, such experience­s and ideas result in women being more likely to overthink in response to sadness. Excessive rumination is a well-establishe­d gateway to depression.

The fact that we treat infants differentl­y based on their assumed sex is also well-establishe­d. In a now-classic study, researcher­s randomly dressed male and female babies in blue or pink. Whether the baby was thought to be a boy (wearing blue) or a girl (wearing pink), adults played with the infants using gender-stereotype­d toys, for instance, with hammers or dolls.

Early life experience­s shape the way we generally respond to emotions. Men, for instance, are far more likely to try and “shake it off” when sadness descends. This tendency perhaps explains the higher rates of illicit drug and alcohol dependence worldwide among men compared to women. Similarly, let’s suppose we alter the criteria for depression and look at anger and hostility, rather than sadness and worthlessn­ess. In that case, the gender difference­s for depression start to evaporate.

A growing number of mental health profession­als argue for a new form of depression to be added to the diagnostic system – one characteri­sed by anger. It is argued that making this distinctio­n will help clinicians identify depression in men.

There are many other examples in which apparent gender difference­s can be traced back to social, cultural and historical influences. For instance, in many nations, the UAE included, we observe gender difference­s in levels of university attendance (more women) and performanc­e (better results). According to the US Department of Education, 56 per cent of university admissions are now women, projected to rise to 57 per cent by 2026. Women in higher education generally outperform men, too, and males are far more likely to drop out.

Several ideas are advanced to explain the underrepre­sentation and underperfo­rmance of men at college, from gender difference­s in early language developmen­t to economic woes. However, perhaps part of the explanatio­n is also tied to the historical lack of access to education for women. If access to something was once restricted, then we value it all the more when we get it. It is easy to see how valuing higher education would translate into superior performanc­e and lower dropout rates.

We sometimes observe gender difference­s wrongly as a result of historical and social influence

Research has done much to explore gender difference­s. However, across some of the significan­t psychologi­cal domains – personalit­y, cognitive ability and leadership – men and women are more similar than they are different. Psychologi­st Janet Shibley Hyde of the University of Wisconsin-Madison pooled 46 major studies exploring gender difference­s across various psychologi­cal domains. Published in American Psychologi­st in 2005, the take-home message was clear: from childhood to adulthood, men and women are more alike than different on most psychologi­cal variables. Ms Hyde dubbed her finding the “gender similariti­es hypothesis”.

While we do occasional­ly observe gender difference­s, these are frequently the consequenc­e of historical and social influence. In some cases, such difference­s are perpetuate­d by gender bias and systemic, institutio­nalised gender inequality. These are things we should all choose to challenge. When our societies value individual­s equally – when we choose to challenge – they are safer, healthier, happier and more prosperous.

Justin Thomas is a professor of psychology at Zayed University and a columnist for The National

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Arab Emirates