Daily Mail

Sleep better and remembe MORE!

It’s quality AND quantity of sleep that help us to learn and store memories. And, as a world-leading neuroscien­tist reveals, if you don’t get enough, your brain won’t ‘cleanse’ itself and you could face a bigger risk of dementia

- by Professor Matthew Walker

IN SATURDAY’S Mail, in the first extract from his brilliant new book, neuroscien­tist Matthew Walker explained why sleep really is the best medicine, protecting us against cancer and heart disease. Today, Liverpool-born Matthew, who is now professor of neuroscien­ce and psychology at the University of California, Berkeley, and director of the Centre for Human Sleep Science, explores the connection between sleep, memory and Alzheimer’s . . .

The two most feared diseases throughout developed nations are dementia and cancer. Both are related to inadequate sleep. In Saturday’s Mail, I explained how a lack of sleep is connected to cancer. But it is also becoming recognised as a key lifestyle factor determinin­g whether or not you will develop Alzheimer’s disease.

Sleep quality — especially that of deep, NREM or non-rapid eye movement sleep, which is the sleep we have in the first part of the night — naturally deteriorat­es as we age, which is linked to a decline in memory.

however, if you assess a patient with Alzheimer’s disease, the disruption of deep sleep is far more exaggerate­d. More telling, perhaps, is the fact that sleep disturbanc­e precedes the onset of Alzheimer’s by several years, suggesting that it may be an early warning sign of the condition, or even a contributo­r to it.

Alzheimer’s disease is associated with the build-up of a toxic form of protein called beta-amyloid, which aggregates in sticky clumps, or plaques, in the brain. The amyloid plaques kill brain cells. But the amyloid plaques only affect some parts of the brain and not others, for reasons which remain unclear.

What struck me about this unexplaine­d pattern was the location in the brain where amyloid accumulate­s during the early stages of Alzheimer’s and most severely in the later stages. It is the middle of the frontal lobe — which is essential for the generation of deep, non-rapid eye movement sleep.

I joined forces with my colleague Dr William Jagust, a leading authority on Alzheimer’s disease at the University of California, Berkeley. We discovered that the more amyloid deposits there were in the middle regions of the frontal lobe, the more impaired the deep sleep quality.

We are now examining whether this particular ‘dent’ in sleeping brainwave activity represents an early identifier of those who are at greatest risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease, years in advance.

Further studies in mice show that a lack of deep sleep also affects what can be called a night-time power cleanse, where cerebrospi­nal fluid bathes the brain to flush out metabolic waste.

Without sufficient sleep, this does not happen, creating a vicious cycle where amyloid plaques prevent deep sleep, and in turn a lack of deep sleep prevents brain cleansing, leading to more build-up of plaques.

From this comes a prediction: getting too little sleep across the adult life span will significan­tly raise your risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease.

By the way (and strictly non-scientific­ally) I have always found it interestin­g that Margaret Thatcher and Ronald Reagan — two heads of state who were very vocal, if not proud, about sleeping four to five hours a night — both went on to develop the disease.

Donald Trump, the current occupant of the White house and also a vociferous proclaimer of sleeping just a few hours each night, may want to take note.

HAVE you ever wondered how your memory works? how is it possible, for example, that you can keep on learning new things, day after day, and still remember them weeks, months or years later? That’s exactly what my team and I set out to discover some years ago.

We found that sleep is absolutely crucial to the process, both before learning (to prepare your brain for making new memories) and after learning (to cement those memories and prevent forgetting).

We began by asking how the brain, which is constantly acquiring informatio­n when awake, manages to retain it.

It is already known that for factbased informatio­n, such as memorising someone’s name or a new phone number, the hippocampu­s — a long, fingershap­ed structure tucked deep on either side of your brain — offers a short-term reservoir, or temporary informatio­n store, for accumulati­ng new memories.

Unfortunat­ely, the hippocampu­s has a limited storage capacity, almost like a roll of camera film. how, then, does the brain deal with this limited storage — and is sleep part of the process?

To find out, my research team began by dividing participan­ts into two groups and each group was set a rigorous learning task: they were given 100 face-name pairs to remember.

The results for each group were comparable. Then one group was allowed a 90-minute nap; the other stayed awake, playing board games or browsing the internet. Afterwards, they were given a different set of 100 face-name pairs to remember.

The group who’d had the 90-minute siesta did markedly better, and actually improved their earlier score, while the group who’d stayed awake became progressiv­ely worse at learning.

The difference between the two groups at 6pm was not small: a 20 per cent learning advantage for those who slept.

Next, we tried to discover exactly what it was about sleep that provided the benefit.

Analysing the electrical brainwaves of those in the nap group brought our answer.

The memory refreshmen­t happened during lighter, stage 2 NREM sleep (nonrapid eye movement), and specifical­ly short, powerful bursts of electrical activity called sleep spindles. The more spindles during the nap, the greater the restoratio­n of learning.

As we analysed the sleep-spindle bursts of activity, we observed a strikingly reliable loop of electrical current pulsing throughout the brain that repeated every 100 to 200 millisecon­ds. The pulses kept weaving a path back and forth between the hippocampu­s, with its short-term, limited storage space, and the larger, longterm storage site of the cortex.

In that moment, we had just become privy to an electrical transactio­n occurring in the quiet secrecy of sleep.

That is, a shifting of fact-based memories from the temporary storage depot (the hippocampu­s) to a long-term secure vault (the cortex). Sleep had cleared out the hippocampu­s, replenishi­ng this short-term informatio­n repository with plentiful free space so that the learning of new facts could begin again.

We and other research groups have since repeated this study across a full night of sleep and replicated the same finding: the more sleep spindles an individual has at night, the greater the restoratio­n of learning ability come the next morning.

The second benefit of sleep for memory is that it effectivel­y clicks the ‘save’ button on those newly created files.

Countless experiment­s in the past 100 years have shown that sleep provides a memory retention benefit of between 20 and 40 per cent, compared with the same amount of time awake — not a trivial amount when you consider studying for an exam, or, in an evolutiona­ry context, being able to remember the locations of food, water and predators.

We obtain most of our deep NREM (non-rapid eye movement) sleep early in the night.

In contrast, we get most of our rapid eye movement (REM) sleep — the stage in which we dream — and lighter NREM sleep late in the night. The different sorts of sleep were discovered in the Fifties, and experiment­s in which

‘It’s interestin­g that Thatcher and Reagan, who slept 4 to 5 hours a night, both developed Alzheimer’s

participan­ts were allowed to sleep only for the first or second half of the night were clear: for fact-based memory, it was early-night sleep, rich in deep non- rapid eye movement, that provided better memory retention.

Investigat­ions in the early 2000s arrived at a similar conclusion. Using MRI scans, we have since looked deep into the brains of participan­ts to see where memories are being retrieved from before sleep, and how that compares with from where they are retrieved after sleep.

Those informatio­n packets were being recalled from very different geographic­al locations within the brain at the two different times.

Before having slept, participan­ts were fetching memories from the short-term storage site of the hippocampu­s — that temporary warehouse. But things looked very different by the next morning. The memories had moved.

After the full night of sleep, participan­ts were retrieving that same informatio­n from the neocortex — a region at the top of the brain that serves as the long-term storage site for fact- based memories — where they can live safely, perhaps in perpetuity.

We had observed a transactio­n that takes place each night when we sleep. The slow brainwaves of deep NREM had served as a courier service, shifting memory packets from a temporary storage hold (hippocampu­s) to a more secure, permanent home (the cortex).

An important point to note here is that this process only works if you get a good night’s sleep the same day that you’ve learned something. If you miss out on that sleep, the memory will not be kept in the same way, even if you try to catch up on subsequent nights. In terms of memory, sleep is not like the bank. If you accumulate a debt, you cannot pay it off later.

Recent studies show that sleep also helps you regain access to memories that you could not retrieve before you went to sleep.

Like a computer hard drive where some files have become corrupted and inaccessib­le, sleep offers a recovery service at night, so that you awake the next morning able to locate and retrieve those once unavailabl­e memory files with ease and precision.

That’s the, ‘ Ah yes, now I remember’ sensation that you may have experience­d after a good night of sleep.

ALL the studies I’ve described so far deal with one type of memory — factual, which we associate with textbooks or rememberin­g someone’s name. There are, however, many other types of memory, including that for skills.

Take riding a bike, for example. There’s no point in reading about how to do it; you can only learn how to ride a bike by doing, which is to say by practising.

The same is true for all motor skills, whether you are learning a musical instrument, an athletic sport, a surgical procedure, or how to fly a plane.

The term ‘muscle memory’ is a misnomer. Muscles themselves have no such memory: a muscle that is not connected to a brain cannot perform any skilled actions, nor does a muscle store skilled routines.

Muscle memory is, in fact, brain memory. Training and strength- ening muscles can help you better execute a memorised routine. But the routine itself — the memory programme — resides firmly and exclusivel­y within the brain.

My studies have shown that practise does not make perfect. It is practise, followed by a night of sleep, that leads to perfection.

In what spanned almost a decade of research, I finally identified the type of sleep responsibl­e for overnight motorskill­s improvemen­t.

They are directly related to the amount of lighter non rapid eye movement sleep, which happens especially in the last two hours of an eight-hour night of sleep (eg, from 5am to 7am, should you have fallen asleep at 11pm). Those last two hours of sleep are precisely the window that many of us decide to abandon in order to get ahead with the day.

As a result, we miss out on the benefits of this last stage of sleep.

ADAPTED from Why We Sleep: The New Science Of Sleep And Dreams by Matthew Walker, published by Penguin Books at £9.99. © Matthew Walker 2018. To buy a copy for £7.99 (20 per cent discount), call 0844 571 0640 or go to mailshop.co.uk/books. P&P is free on orders over £15. Offer valid until August 11, 2018.

 ?? Picture: SHUTTERSTO­CK ??
Picture: SHUTTERSTO­CK

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Kingdom