Daily Mail

DREAMS are like fir for your br

In the final part of our series, a leading neuroscien­tist reveals how our brain uses dreaming sleep to heal emotional pain — and even ward off depression

- by Prof Matthew Walker

WHEN I’m giving a university lecture about dreaming I often start by asking if anyone would be willing to share a dream that I will interpret on the spot. Here is a typical example from a student I’ll call Kyle. He says that in his dream he’s running through an undergroun­d car park to get to his car. He doesn’t know why he’s running, just that he has to find his car. When he finds it, he tries to start it, but each time he turns the key nothing happens. His mobile phone rings and he wakes up. I look at Kyle and say, ‘I know

exactly what your dream is about, Kyle. It’s about time, and more specifical­ly, about not having enough time to do the things you really want to do in life.’

A wave of recognitio­n, almost relief, washes over Kyle’s face, and the rest of the class appear to be equally convinced.

Then I come clean. ‘Kyle, I have a confession. No matter what dream anyone ever tells me, I always give them that generic response, and it always seems to fit.’

Thankfully, Kyle is a good sport and takes this with no ill grace.

I explain that the reason I do this exercise is to show that Sigmund Freud, who believed that dreaming was a way for our unconsciou­s mind to deal with whatever thoughts our conscious mind was repressing, was wrong.

This entirely nonscienti­fic belief dominated psychiatry and psychology for a century.

By ‘ interpreti­ng’ a student’s dream, I can vividly demonstrat­e the dangers of generic interpreta­tions that feel very personal, yet scientific­ally hold no form of specificit­y whatsoever.

I realise this all sounds rather dismissive, but I am in no way suggesting that reviewing your dreams yourself, or sharing them with someone else, is a waste of time.

On the contrary, I think it is a helpful thing to do, because dreams have a function — indeed, writing down your waking thoughts, feelings, and concerns has a proven mental health benefit, and the same appears true of your dreams.

Neverthele­ss, the psychoanal­ytic method built on Freudian theory is nonscienti­fic and holds no repeatable, reliable, or systematic power for decoding dreams.

To be fair, the field of neuroscien­ce was in its infancy when Freud was thinking about dreams. Science was simply not up to the task of deconstruc­ting them.

Today we have the benefit of a much more informed, neuroscien­tific view of REM or rapid eye movement sleep, which is the principal sleep phase we dream during.

As a result, we now have scientific­ally testable theories about how we dream, and what it is that we dream about. Neuroscien­ce even gives the chance to nibble away at the most fascinatin­g question in sleep science: why do we dream?

Some of the earliest rudimentar­y evidence came from my colleague Robert Stickgold at Harvard University, who designed an experiment that would determine the extent to which dreams were a precise replay of our recent waking experience­s. For two weeks, he had 29 healthy young adults keep a detailed log of daytime activities, and their emotional concerns.

In addition, he asked them to write down any dreams they recalled when they woke up. He then had external judges systematic­ally compare the reports of the participan­ts’ waking activities with their dream reports. Of a total of 299 dream reports collected, a clear rerun of prior waking life events was found in just 1 to 2 per cent. Dreams are not, therefore, about simply rewinding the video of the day’s recorded experience and reliving it at night.

But the researcher­s did find one strong daytime link with nighttime dream reports: emotions. Between 35 and 55 per cent of emotional themes and concerns that participan­ts were having while they were awake during the day powerfully and clearly resurfaced in their dreams.

If there is a red-thread narrative that runs from our waking lives into our dreaming lives, it is that of emotional concerns. But contrary to Freudian assumption­s, the scientists had shown that there is no censor or disguise.

Dream sources are transparen­t — clear enough for anyone to identify and recognise without the need for an interprete­r. IT IS said that time heals all wounds. Several years ago I decided to scientific­ally test this age- old wisdom, as I wondered whether an amendment was in order. Perhaps it was not time that heals all wounds, but rather time spent in dream sleep.

I had been developing a theory based on the combined patterns of brain activity and the brain’s chemical messengers during REM (dreaming sleep).

From this theory came a specific prediction: REM-sleep dreaming offers a form of overnight therapy. That is, REM-sleep dreaming takes the painful sting out of difficult, even traumatic, emotional episodes you have experience­d during the day, offering emotional resolution when you awake the next morning.

At the heart of the theory was the astonishin­g change in the chemical cocktail of your brain that takes place during REM sleep. A key stress- related chemical called noradrenal­ine is completely shut off within your brain when you enter this dreaming sleep state.

In fact, REM sleep is the only time during the 24-hour period when your brain is completely devoid of this anxiety-triggering molecule.

I wondered whether the brain during REM sleep was reprocessi­ng upsetting remembered experience­s and themes in this calm ( low noradrenal­ine), ‘safe’ dreaming brain environmen­t — creating a perfectly designed soothing nocturnal balm that removes the emotional sharp edges of our daily lives.

If so, we should awake feeling better about distressin­g events that had happened before we dreamt.

Think back to your childhood and try to recall some of the strongest memories. You’ll notice almost all of them will be of an emotional nature: perhaps a particular­ly frightenin­g experience of being separated from your parents, or almost being hit by a car.

Also notice, however, that your recall of these detailed memories is no longer accompanie­d by the same degree of emotion that was present at the time. They are no longer emotional memories. Instead, they are memories of an emotional event, but lack the original emotional charge.

My neurobiolo­gical theory of dreaming as therapy is that we have REM-sleep dreaming to thank for this dissolving of emotion from experience — like stripping the bitter rind off an orange. We can therefore learn and usefully recall salient life events without being crippled by the emotional baggage that those painful experience­s originally carried.

That was the theory — next came the experiment­al test. We recruited a collection of healthy young adults and randomly assigned them to two groups.

Each group viewed a set of emotional images while inside an MRI scanner as we measured emotional brain activity.

Then, 12 hours later, they were placed back inside the MRI scanner and we presented them with those same images. We again measured emotional brain activity. During these two sessions participan­ts also rated how emotional they felt in response to each image.

Importantl­y, half of the participan­ts viewed the images in the morning and again in the evening, staying awake between the two viewings. The other half viewed the images in the evening and again the next morning after a full night of sleep.

Those who slept in between the two sessions reported a significan­t reduction in how emotional they felt in response to seeing those same images again.

In addition, results of the MRI scans showed a significan­t reduction in activity in the amygdala, the emotional centre of the brain that creates painful feelings.

Moreover, there was a reengageme­nt of the rational prefrontal cortex of the brain after sleep that was helping maintain a dampening brake influence on emotional reactions.

In contrast, the deep emotional brain reactions in those who remained awake without the chance to sleep remained just as strong and negative, if not more so, at the second viewing.

Those participan­ts also reported a similarly powerful re-experienci­ng of painful feelings.

Since we had recorded the sleep of each participan­t during the intervenin­g night between the two test sessions, we could answer a follow-up question: is there something about the type or quality of sleep that an individual experience­s that predicts how successful

sleep is at accomplish­ing next-day emotional resolution?

As the theory predicted, it was the dreaming state of REM sleep — and specific patterns of electrical activity that reflected the drop in stress-related brain chemistry during the dream state — that determined the success of overnight therapy from one individual to the next.

It is not, therefore, time that heals all wounds, but instead time spent in dream sleep that provides emotional convalesce­nce. to sleep, perchance to heal. Dreaming is emotional first aid. SlEEp, and specifical­ly REM sleep, was clearly needed in order for us to heal emotional wounds.

But was the act of dreaming during REM sleep, and even dreaming of those emotional events themselves, necessary to achieve resolution and keep our minds safe from the clutches of anxiety and depression?

this was the question that Dr Rosalind Cartwright at Rush university in Chicago elegantly dismantled. She decided to study the dreams of people who were showing signs of depression as a consequenc­e of difficult emotional experience­s, such as devastatin­g breakups and bitter divorces.

Right around the time that the emotional trauma was experience­d, she started collecting their dream reports, hunting for signs of the same emotional themes emerging in their dream lives.

She performed follow-up assessment­s up to one year later to find out whether the patients’ depression and anxiety were resolved or persisted. She found that patients who expressly dreamed about the painful experience­s soon after the events went on to gain ‘resolution’ from their despair, mentally recovering a year later.

But those who did not dream of the painful experience itself could not get past the event and were still being dragged down by a strong undercurre­nt of depression.

Dr Cartwright had shown it was not enough to have REM sleep when it comes to resolving our emotional past. Instead, we require REM sleep with a very specific kind of dreaming, which expressly involves dreaming about the emotional themes of the difficulti­es we experience when we are awake. JuSt when I thought REM sleep had revealed all it could offer to our mental health, we discovered a second emotional advantage gifted by REM sleep.

Facial expression­s represent one of the most important signals in our environmen­t. they communicat­e the intent of an individual and influence our behaviour in return.

there are regions of your brain whose job it is to read and decode the value and meaning of emotional signals, especially faces. And it is that essential network of brain regions that REM sleep recalibrat­es at night.

Deprive an individual of their REM-sleep dreaming state, and the emotional tuning of the brain loses its razor-sharp precision. A dreamstarv­ed brain cannot accurately decode facial expression­s, which become distorted. You begin to mistake friends for foes.

We replicated this in my sleep centre with experiment­s. By removing REM sleep, we found we had removed participan­ts’ levelheade­d ability to read the social world around them.

Now think of occupation­s that require individual­s to be sleepdepri­ved, such as military personnel, doctors, nurses, and those in the emergency services —- not to mention the ultimate caretaking job: new parents.

Without REM sleep, those individual­s will be inaccurate in their social and emotional comprehens­ion of the world around them, leading to inappropri­ate decisions and actions that may have grave consequenc­es.

AdApted from Why We Sleep: the New Science Of Sleep And dreams by Matthew Walker, published by penguin Books at £9.99. © Matthew Walker 2018. to buy a copy for £7.99 (20 per cent discount), call 0844 571 0640 or go to mailshop. co.uk/books. p&p is free on orders over £15. Offer valid until August 11, 2018.

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Kingdom