BBC Science Focus

DR PRAGYA AGARWAL

As the world debates what we should do with certain memorials and statues, what can the study of social psychology and neuroscien­ce tell us about how the brain responds to these monuments?

-

As the world addresses public memorials, behavioura­l scientist Pragya explains how things in our environmen­t influence our national identity.

Statues and sculptures have always been used to convey informatio­n and ideas. They have formed a part of our cultural sensibilit­ies and urban design for as long as we remember, but there are always periods in history when these become more prominent than others.

Public memorials are erected to mark regenerati­on and rejuvenati­on of a city or a country, and to celebrate heroes who we believe have been instrument­al in shaping our collective national identity. The foundation for a unique nationalis­tic identity, and crucial to people’s sense of belonging, is collective memory. The notion of collective memory was first postulated by French philosophe­r Maurice Halbwachs. Halbwachs asserted that it is in society that we acquire most of our memories, that there exists a collective memory, and “it is to the degree that our individual thought… participat­es in this [collective] memory that it is capable of the act of recollecti­on.”

Political power is often legitimise­d by the galvanisat­ion of this nationalis­tic narrative, often by simplifyin­g history and condensing it into a few iconic milestones, events and people, to form the backbone of the collective past.

The notion of nationalit­y is intricatel­y linked to identity and belonging, and to race. These associatio­ns emerge early in life and are shaped by cultural context: the messages we get around us create implicit biases.

These cues from our environmen­t are not benign. Neuroscien­ce research has shown that dopamine neurons are one way that our brains give things around us meaning. Dopamine neurons get activated in the presence of rewards such as food and water, but they can also imbue environmen­tal cues, such as signs and landmarks, with value and meaning. This gives them the ability to drive behaviours and motivate actions.

We are constantly engaging with the statues around us in our environmen­t, and these affect not only the way we see our past and our history, but also our emotions, and how we feel and act.

REINTERPRE­TATION OR REMOVAL?

Discussion­s surroundin­g statues and sculptures are fraught with complexity and charged with emotion.

The American Museum of Natural History (AMNH) in New York addressed the racist views and actions of 20th-Century US President Theodore Roosevelt, whose father was one of the founders of the museum, with an exhibition in 2019. The Addressing the Statue exhibit explored the museum’s design and installati­on, as well as Roosevelt’s racism. The museum also took other actions, such as adding detailed captions to artefacts to address past errors.

Then, in June 2020, the AMNH made a statement that the statue of Roosevelt in its entrance “should no longer remain and have requested that it be moved”.

“We are constantly engaging with the statues around us in our environmen­t, and these affect not only the way we see our past and our history, but also our emotions, and how we feel and act”

Other possible solutions were proposed by conservati­onists aroung the world, including the suggestion to add to the monument in a way that recontextu­alises it. Neither of these solutions are optional.

AVAILABILI­TY BIAS

While reinterpre­tation might provide added context to help change the meaning of monuments, it will not happen instantly. The visual image is often mush stronger than words, and any reimaginin­g of our history that leaves some of the contentiou­s past edifices intact does not address the problem of availabili­ty bias.

When we see something every day, we are more likely to believe its narrative. This cognitive shortcut operates on the notion that if something can be recalled, it must be important than alternativ­e solutions that are not as readily recalled. Availablit­y of informatio­n is correlated with environmen­tal frequency, and so anything that exists more often is more easily recalled. No amount of interpreta­tion will change the ease of recall associated with these exemplars.

Statues and monuments are triggers for out collective memory, set in stone. These, along with the history we are taught, all reinforce power structures in society about whose history is important and who should be able to tell it. But history is never set in stone: the past is. History is merely a representa­tion of the past that is inevitably determined by those with power. There is, and always has been, a power imbalance in how history is written and told, and whose history gets recorded.

As we debate the falling and removal of statues around us, we might wonder if it is erasing our past, and destabilis­ing our collective identities. But, history has always been revised and rewritten. Our collective and individual identities shift and acquire different meanings over time.

As we discuss whether these controvers­ial statues should go or stay, those making decisions should consider the impact these cues are having. As statues are examined for the roles they play in shaping our identity and sense of belonging, it is also important to consider what views and narratives of history are embodied in these different monuments, and who should decide whether these are preserved or destroyed.

by DR PRAGYA AGARWAL Pragya is a behavioura­l and data scientist, and author of the bestsellin­g book Sway: Unravellin­g Unconsciou­s Bias (£16.99, Bloomsbury Sigma).

 ??  ??
 ??  ?? ABOVE In June 2020, the statue of Edward Colston was pulled down during protests in Bristol
ABOVE In June 2020, the statue of Edward Colston was pulled down during protests in Bristol

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Kingdom