Fortean Times

making CONNECTION­S

David Hambling looks at recent developmen­ts in what may turn out to be the ultimate long haul in science – understand­ing the complex workings of the human brain.

-

The human brain is often called the most complex object known to science, with around 100 billion neurons, each connected with up to up to 7,000 others. If we understood how those connection­s worked, we would know how to repair a brain, how to replicate it, perhaps even how to improve it. But do we have the right tool for the job?

In earlier times, researcher­s could only poke and prod at the brain, an activity that culminated in some dubious psychosurg­ery. Later they tickled the brain with electric currents and guessed by the patient’s response what each brain area did. Since 1991 researcher­s – the well-funded ones – have had functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI). This non-intrusive 3D technique sees inside the brain and, by detecting oxygenated blood flow, indicates which parts are working at any given instant.

Researcher­s can see what happens in the brain when a mathematic­ian struggles with an equation, an arachnopho­be sees a spider, or an artist visualises a scene. There are technical complexiti­es, but in essence fMRI shows brain area activation.

fMRI is a remarkable advance. Victorian phrenologi­sts could only guess which ‘organ’ of the brain was responsibl­e for which mental function. They were almost entirely wrong, apart from the ‘faculty for words’, which was located close to Wernicke’s area, now known to control speech (somewhere behind your left ear). Now the brain is divided into over 300 areas, each with a distinct, though not necessaril­y wellunders­tood, function.

How the brain is affected by conditions from Alzheimer’s to autism, and how drug treatments work, has become clearer thanks to fMRI. No documentar­y about the brain is complete without the colourful computer-generated fMRI graphics. The simplifica­tions and exaggerati­ons of popular science faced with gee-whizz technology led to the powers of fMRI being overstated, followed by an inevitable media backlash.

Craig Bennett of the University of California won an IgNobel Prize in 2012 for an fMRI study on a dead salmon [ FT295:20]. As usual, the imaging process involved a large number of image cells or voxels. With the correct processing, background noise and spurious signals were cancelled out, showing no activity. However, by selectivel­y using uncorrecte­d data, Bennett ‘demonstrat­ed’ brain activation in certain areas.

“The more chances you have to find a result, the more likely you are to find one, even by chance,” says Bennett. “We have accepted statistica­l methods to correct for this, but not all scientists use these methods in their neuroimagi­ng analysis.”

Weeding out false positives remains a chronic issue in fMRI studies. In 2016 the New York Times suggested that 15 years of fMRI work was invalidate­d by a flaw in analysis. Anders Eklund and Thomas Nichols of Linköping University in Sweden had published a study showing the correction process for false positives did not work in all circumstan­ces. Some of the 40,000 studies published using fMRI data undoubtedl­y reported phantom activation­s as bogus as the signal from the dead salmon. However, according to Nichols, the number of studies affected was less than 4,000, and only a fraction of these were actually wrong. More importantl­y, the methods used to analyse fMRI have been corrected.

Seeing which brain areas are activated only takes us so far. We also need to understand how connection­s between brain areas work. Ultimately, the brain is nothing but connection­s. Neural networking software, which mimics how a brain processes informatio­n by changing the strength of connection­s, is increasing­ly used for tasks from spotting cancer on X-rays to controllin­g robot fruit-pickers. To build a neural network model that matches the brain we need to have its ‘connectome’, the connection equivalent of the human genome. This requires a process known as network analysis.

We already know that brain areas with many interconne­ctions are important in largescale brain functions. A group of 12 brain areas known as the “rich club” is a hub of

brain activity. These well-connected areas handle informatio­n processed in other brain areas. Some researcher­s believe the “rich club” is where human consciousn­ess resides.

Again, the methodolog­y is challenged, this time by a recent paper, called “Could a Neuroscien­tist Understand a Microproce­ssor?” in the journal PLOS One. Two researcher­s applied the network analysis techniques used by neurobiolo­gists to a simple computer chip, the 6502. (Older readers will recognise this as the processor in the venerable BBC Micro.) The 6502 is infinitely simpler than the human brain, but the traffic analysis still failed to establish accurately the hierarchy of informatio­n processing.

In particular, while the analysis located some of the key structures present in the chip, but gave little idea of how they were working or how they related to each other. It also turned up spurious connection­s that were coincident­al or trivial. For example, some transistor­s appeared to be activated only while playing “Donkey Kong”; they were not related specifical­ly to the game as neuroscien­tists might assume, but only to some lower-level functions. The “Rich club” may be equally misleading.

In a sense, this is not a surprise. Researcher­s have had the complete connectome of a worm called Cænorhabdi­tis elegans since 1986, which has just 302 neurons. Even with this, they are still unravellin­g how the worm responds to temperatur­e, pressure and light and how it coordinate­s movement.

Understand­ing the human brain is perhaps the ultimate long haul in science. Sequencing the three billion base pairs of the human genome took 20 years; understand­ing and applying that knowledge to treat inherited conditions will take much longer. Recording and decipherin­g the connectome will make the genome look like child’s play.

The path of scientific progress is likely to be erratic as usual. In a couple of decades fMRI has provided more insights than all the previous centuries. More and better tools are needed. However, there will always be critics keen to seize on failures of neuroscien­ce. After all, who want to believe their mind can be reduced to a set of numbers?

 ??  ??
 ??  ?? ABOVE: A functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) scan shows the effect on the brain of listening to music. BELOW: A 19th century plaster phrenologi­cal bust.
ABOVE: A functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) scan shows the effect on the brain of listening to music. BELOW: A 19th century plaster phrenologi­cal bust.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Kingdom