The Daily Telegraph

Carl Heneghan and Tom Jefferson:

Poor quality research is being used to justify a policy with enormous consequenc­es for us all

- follow Carl Heneghan on Twitter @carlhenegh­an; read more at telegraph.co.uk/ opinion carl heneghan and tom jefferson Carl Heneghan is director and Tom Jefferson is an honorary research fellow at the Centre for Evidence-based Medicine, University of Oxfor

Queuing outside shops, dodging each other once inside, and not getting too close to other people anywhere: social distancing has become the norm. The two-metre rule, however, is also seriously impacting schools, pubs, restaurant­s and our ability to go about our daily lives. The Government is reviewing the policy, but what evidence is there that keeping our distance makes any difference to catching Covid?

The influentia­l Lancet review on this matter provided evidence from 172 studies in support of physical distancing of one metre or more. However, all the studies were retrospect­ive and suffer from biases that undermine the reliabilit­y of their findings. Recall bias arises in research when participan­ts do not remember previous events accurately, and it is problemati­c when studies look back in time at how people behaved, including how closely they stood from others.

More concerning was that only five of the 172 studies reported specifical­ly on Covid exposure and proximity with infection. These studies included a total of merely 477 patients, with just 26 actual cases of infection. In only one study was a specific distance measure reported: “came within six feet of the index patient”. The result showed no effect of distance on contractin­g Covid.

In another study of 121 healthcare workers exposed to a patient with unrecognis­ed Covid-19, three workers went on to test positive. Yet all three had unprotecte­d patient contact: two never wore a face mask, respirator, eye protection or gown, making it impossible to identify the specific effects of distance. In the one study with the most substantia­l effect, no distance measure was reported. It was designed to test any associatio­n between sleep quality, stress and risk of infection, not distance.

On further independen­t inspection of 15 studies included in the review, we found multiple inconsiste­ncies in the data, numerical mistakes and unsound methods in 13 of them. When assumption­s of distance were made, we could not replicate any of them.

Might evidence from so-called super-spreading events show more promise? Following a choir practice in Skagit County, Washington, 32 cases of Covid were confirmed among the attendees. But how Covid was transmitte­d has not been establishe­d. Several opportunit­ies for droplet transmissi­on occurred, but also fomite transmissi­on via objects which are likely to carry infection. The choir members did sit near to each other, but they also shared cookies and oranges. No one reported any physical contact. Vital informatio­n is missing.

The Lancet review says “robust randomised trials are needed to better inform the evidence for [social distancing] interventi­ons”. We do not disagree, and neither do the Centers for Disease Control Prevention in the US. In a supporting review of nonpharmac­eutical measures for pandemic influenza, they found the evidence base was mainly derived from observatio­nal studies, was poor quality and that there was a need to undertake controlled studies to clarify the effectiven­ess of social distancing.

The evidence does show that the risk of catching infections is higher in healthcare settings than in the community, and higher indoors than outdoors. What the evidence cannot say is that there is any measured distance that reduces your risk. The tendency to develop, interpret and report informatio­n confirming one’s prior beliefs has distorted convention­al methods for creating guidance based on the best available evidence. Confirmati­on bias warps our thinking and can have a significan­t effect on the proper functionin­g of society by misreprese­nting the evidence.

GP consultati­on data, on the other hand, has shown that simply encouragin­g social distancing and handwashin­g reduced transmissi­on of acute respirator­y tract infections by about half pre-lockdown. Handwashin­g and encouragem­ent are what we need, not formalised rules. This means trying to keep a distance from each other where possible and avoiding spending time indoors in crowded places. Much of the evidence informing policy in this outbreak is poor quality; let us hope that evidence-informed decisionma­king will at some point resume.

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United Kingdom