Arkansas Democrat-Gazette

Woman’s blindness a curious brain case

After years in psychother­apy, some of her alternate personalit­ies can see

- SARAH KAPLAN

It had been more than a decade since “B.T.” had last seen anything.

After suffering a traumatic accident as a young woman, doctors diagnosed her with cortical blindness, caused by damage to the visual processing centers in her brain. So she got a guide dog and grew accustomed to the darkness.

Besides, B.T. had other health problems to cope with — namely, more than 10 wildly different personalit­ies that competed for control of her body. It was while seeking treatment for her dissociati­ve identity disorder that the ability to see suddenly returned. Not to B.T., a 37-yearold German woman. But to a teenage boy she sometimes became.

With therapy, over the course of months, all but two of B.T.’s identities regained their sight. And as B.T. oscillated between identities, her vision flicked on and off like a light switch in her mind. The world would appear, then go dark.

Writing in PsyCh Journal, B.T.’s doctors say her blindness wasn’t caused by brain damage, her original diagnosis. It was instead something more akin to a brain directive, a psychologi­cal problem rather than a physiologi­cal one.

B.T.’s strange case reveals a lot about the mind’s extraordin­ary power — how it can control what we see and who we are.

To understand what happened with B.T. (who is identified only by her initials in the journal article), her doctors, German psychologi­sts Hans Strasburge­r and Bruno Waldvogel, went all the way back to her initial diagnosis of cortical blindness.

Her health records from the time show that she was subjected to a series of vision tests involving lasers, special glasses, lights shined across a room, all of which demonstrat­ed her apparent blindness. Since there was no damage to her eyes themselves, it was assumed that B.T.’s vision problems must have come from brain damage caused by her accident (the report does not say what exactly happened in the accident).

Waldvogel had no reason to doubt that diagnosis when B.T. was referred to him 13 years later for treatment for dissociati­ve identity disorder, once called multiple personalit­y disorder. B.T. exhibited more than 10 personalit­ies, each of them varying in age, sex, habits and temperamen­t. They even spoke different languages: some communicat­ed only in English, others only in German, some in both. B.T. had spent time in an English-speaking country as a child but lived in Germany.

Then, four years into psychother­apy, something strange happened: Just after ending a therapy session, while in one of her adolescent male states, B.T. saw a word on the cover of a magazine. It was the first word she had read visually in 17 years.

At first, B.T.’s renewed sight was restricted to recognizin­g whole words in that one identity. If asked, she couldn’t even see the individual letters that made up the words, just the words themselves. But it gradually expanded, first to higher-order visual processes — like reading — then to lower-level ones — like recognizin­g patterns — until most of her personalit­ies were able to see most of the time. When B.T. alternated between sighted and sightless personalit­ies, her vision switched as well.

That’s when Waldvogel began doubting the cause of B.T.’s vision loss. It’s unlikely that a brain injury of the kind that can cause cortical blindness would heal instantane­ously after such a long time. And even if it did, that didn’t explain why B.T.’s vision continued to switch on and off. Clearly something else was going on.

One explanatio­n, that B.T. was “malingerin­g,” or lying about her disability, was disproved by an EEG test. When B.T. was in her two blind states, her brain showed none of the electrical responses to visual stimuli that sighted people would display, even though B.T.’s eyes were open and she was looking right at them.

Instead, Waldvogel and Strasburge­r believe that B.T.’s blindness is psychogeni­c — psychologi­cally caused, rather than physical. Something happened, perhaps related to her accident, that caused her body to react by cutting out her ability to see. Even now, two of her identities retain that coping mechanism.

“These presumably serve as a possibilit­y for retreat,” Strasburge­r told the neuroscien­ce site Brain Decoder. “In situations that are particular­ly emotionall­y intense, the patient occasional­ly feels the wish to become blind, and thus not ‘need to see.’”

It’s not actually all that uncommon for people’s brains to stop them from seeing, the researcher­s say. When your two eyes see slightly different images, when squinting, for example, the brain will cut out one image to keep you from getting confused by the contradict­ion. Your brain also intervenes in visual processing when you focus on particular objects in your field of vision.

Responsibi­lity for the informatio­n “gatekeepin­g” that kept B.T. from seeing everything she looked at may lie with the lateral geniculate nucleus, a sort of neural relay center that sends visual informatio­n down synaptic pathways into the brain’s informatio­n processors.

Perhaps more interestin­g than what it says about sight, though, is what B.T.’s story reveals about dissociati­ve identity disorder, the condition apparently at the root of her vision loss.

Though the disorder has been listed since 1994 in psychiatry’s “bible” — the Diagnostic and Statistica­l Manual of Mental Disorders — there is still a large amount of skepticism about the diagnosis. For years before it became a psychiatri­c diagnosis, dissociati­ve identity disorder was known along with a host of other psychiatri­c conditions as “hysteria.”

Modern critics of the diagnosis point out the absence of consensus on diagnostic criteria and treatment, and blame sensationa­l stories of dissociati­ve identity disorder patients like the 1976 TV movie Sybil for creating an epidemic of multiple personalit­y disorder diagnoses.

The 1990s saw a spate of lawsuits from patients subjected to dubious treatments for multiple personalit­y disorder that they said they didn’t have, and many began to believe that dissociati­ve identity disorder was not so much treated by psychiatri­sts but induced by them through the power of suggestion.

At the very least, it’s thought that dissociati­ve identity disorder may be a product of fragmentat­ion at high levels of thinking, a breakdown in a brain dealing with complex emotions.

But Strasburge­r and Waldvogel say their finding is evidence that the disorder can unfold at a very basic, biological level. After all, it was not just high-level cognitive functions, like reading, that were affected by B.T.’s condition. Even basic things like depth perception were difficult for her. And B.T.’s doctors could see all that playing out in her brain right in front of them on the EEG.

The case study shows that dissociati­ve identity disorder “is a legitimate psycho-physiologi­cally based syndrome of psychologi­cal distress,” Dr. Richard P. Kluft, a clinical professor of psychiatry at Temple University School of Medicine, not associated with the study, told Brain Decoder. The condition is not just a product of culture and psychiatri­sts’ suggestion­s, he said. In B.T.’s case, it “represents the mind’s attempt to compartmen­talize its pain.”

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States