Arkansas Democrat-Gazette

Is monovision a traffic hazard?

- TOM AVRIL

While bifocal eyeglasses allow near and far vision in both eyes, some people opt to split the difference — correcting one eye with a contact lens to sharpen the focus on far-away objects, the other eye with a lens to see things close up.

Called monovision, this way of seeing the world is not for everyone, in part because it can interfere with depth perception.

It turns out that when an object is moving, the effect on depth perception is much worse than is commonly appreciate­d, according to a new study led by a University of Pennsylvan­ia scientist. Monovision creates an optical illusion.

The reason for this optical illusion takes a bit of explanatio­n, but it is significan­t enough that it could pose a public safety hazard, said neuroscien­tist Johannes Burge, an assistant professor in Penn’s psychology department.

Picture a motorist who slows down at an intersecti­on because a cyclist is riding from left to right in cross-traffic. If the driver is wearing monovision lenses and the bike is traveling 15 mph, it will seem to be as much as 9 feet farther away from the car than it actually is, Burge and his colleagues reported in Current Biology.

That illusion could lead the motorist to engage in what the study authors termed “casual braking” rather than coming to a full stop — and hitting the cyclist as a result.

What’s more, the illusion becomes even greater when the bike (or other vehicle in cross-traffic) is traveling faster.

“This illusion is new to science,” Burge said. “It has the potential to cause a pretty serious public safety issue.”

10 MILLION USERS

The findings were based on hours of measuremen­ts made with a haploscope, a finely calibrated contraptio­n in Burge’s lab that consists of eyepieces, mirrors and computer screens. He was joined in the research by two scientists from the Institute of Optics in Madrid, Carlos Dorronsoro and graduate student Victor Rodriguez-Lopez.

An estimated 10 million people in the United States have their eyesight corrected to achieve monovision, typically with contacts or lens implants that are inserted during cataract surgery. Presidents Ford, Carter and Reagan all did so.

Monovision lenses have long been known to cause a slight decline in depth perception because, as the name suggests, they compromise the person’s ability to see in stereo. That’s because a given object appears sharp in one eye and blurry in the other, so the brain suppresses the blurry image to some degree. The viewer is, therefore, less able to “triangulat­e,” and as a result is less precise at telling how far away an object is.

The phenomenon described in the study occurs on top of that loss of precision, but only when the object is in motion. To a person with such lenses, a sideways-moving object appears significan­tly nearer or farther than it actually is, depending on whether it is traveling left to right or right to left.

So a pendulum in a grandfathe­r clock, for example, will appear to be traveling along the path of an oval — weirdly seeming to swing forward and backward as it moves from side to side.

BRAIN PROCESSING SPEED

The reason for this illusion is far from intuitive, but it has to do with the fact that the brain can process blurry images several millisecon­ds faster than it handles sharply focused images, the study authors said.

Wills Eye Hospital surgeon Robert S. Bailey Jr., who was not involved with the research, agreed that it was a novel contributi­on to the field of vision science.

He said the type of illusion the researcher­s identified may be fairly subtle, as none of his patients with monovision lenses had ever mentioned it. Instead, some say it is hard to get used to having objects appear blurry in one eye and focused in the other, said Bailey, chief of the cataract and primary eye care service at Wills Eye.

“It can be hard for the brain to adjust to the fact that you’ve got two different focal points,” he said, but “most people can get used to it.”

Andi Coyle, a resident of Philadelph­ia’s Bella Vista neighborho­od who had monovision lenses implanted during cataract surgery, likewise said she noticed no loss in depth perception. If there is any loss, she said, it is worth it because she does not have to worry about eyeglasses.

“I think it depends upon the individual and how well you can or can’t adapt to it,” she said. “It is so incredibly convenient not having to put your glasses on and off.”

The impact on depth perception is neverthele­ss unmistakab­le in the controlled setting of Burge’s lab.

Rodriguez-Lopez, who is conducting research in the lab while visiting from Spain, set up a computer program one day in July to display a white bar on a screen. To the naked eye, the bar appeared to be moving from side to side. But when seen through monovision lenses, it seemed to emerge and recede as it traveled sideways.

Monovision’s impact on depth perception has been blamed in at least one serious crash. In October 1996, a Delta flight struck a light tower on approach to LaGuardia Airport, in part because the pilot was wearing that type of contacts, federal investigat­ors found.

Was the problem that the lenses made his depth perception less precise, as ophthalmol­ogists at the time were aware? Or was it the result of the added optical illusion reported this July by Burge and his colleagues? Hard to say.

If it was the latter, Burge has an idea to prevent any future such accidents.

Earlier research has shown that the brain processes dark images more slowly than light images. So if one of the lenses used to achieve monovision were tinted by just the right amount, that would cancel out the brain’s faster processing of blurry images, Burge said. The optical illusion would disappear.

He and his colleagues already have demonstrat­ed that this concept works in the lab, and have filed for a provisiona­l patent.

But the pure science of discoverin­g the illusion was enough to delight the Penn researcher. Since the optical illusion is caused when the brain processes the image from one eye a few millisecon­ds slower than the other, that suggests that under ordinary conditions, the brain processes the two images simultaneo­usly.

“The fact that we typically see things quite accurately is evidence that under normal circumstan­ces, they’re calibrated to within less than a millisecon­d,” he said. “That I find kind of wonderful.”

 ?? The Philadelph­ia Inquirer/DAVID MAIALETTI/TNS ?? Neuroscien­tist Johannes Burge demonstrat­es a haploscope July 26 in the Goddard Lab at the University of Pennsylvan­ia, Philadelph­ia.
The Philadelph­ia Inquirer/DAVID MAIALETTI/TNS Neuroscien­tist Johannes Burge demonstrat­es a haploscope July 26 in the Goddard Lab at the University of Pennsylvan­ia, Philadelph­ia.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States