Baltimore Sun Sunday

MEDICINE&SCIENCE

- Jonathan.pitts@baltsun.com

More at developmen­t.

Wang and Lisa Feigenson, the study’s senior author and a professor in the psychology and brain sciences department, have long been interested in the relationsh­ip between primary cognitive skills and those that are learned.

Earlier research by Feigenson and team member Justin Halberda, also a professor in the department, had shown that children who demonstrat­e exceptiona­l approximat­e number sense tend to excel in school-based math. But no one had explored whether manipulati­ng the first affects the second.

The team devised a computer game that exercises the innate number sense of children. For the experiment, they had 40 children play the game and then tested how it affected their math performanc­e.

They showed each child a series of split-screen pictures that included blue dots on one side and yellow dots on the other. The 5-year-olds were asked to judge, without counting, whether there were more blue or yellow dots on each screen.

Working at the Johns Hopkins Laboratory for Child Developmen­t, the researcher­s showed one group of the children the most difficult screens first and worked their way toward the easiest. A second group saw the hardest and easiest screens in random order. A third saw the easiest screens first and were led progressiv­ely to the hardest — a sequence that best approximat­es the way in which we learn.

After proceeding through the game, each student took the same math quiz derived from a standardiz­ed math ability assessment test. They were asked to count backward, write down numbers, solve simple word problems and more.

The third group scored the highest, with about 80 percent correct answers — suggesting that proceeding in the proper order through the primary-skill exercises leads to better math performanc­e.

The hardest-to-easiest group performed most poorly, with 60 percent correct.

The children also took a verbal test, but none showed improvemen­t in that area.

Wang, who initiated the study as a first-year doctoral student three years ago, said the team had no idea what to expect and was stunned by the clarity of the results.

“It was really surprising, even to all of us, that this simple method would actually work,” she said.

However interestin­g, the researcher­s said the findings generated many more questions than the study, supported by the National Institutes of Health, could address.

For instance, how long does such improvemen­t last? Would younger or older children have the same outcomes? Would adults? Would a larger study with more children yield subtler results?

And in the longer run, can computer games like this one be developed to help students of all ages sharpen their computatio­nal abilities?

Wang and her team are following up. They’ve already completed a similar study on a group of 6-month-olds — eliminatin­g any possibilit­y that they’ve been “tainted” by mathematic­al knowledge — with the results to be published later this year.

Those findings, she said, only affirm that the human mind is quantitati­vely very active long before it’s exposed to math concepts — and that it makes sense to explore this poorly understood function further.

“There are a million questions to be asked,” Wang said. “We’re trying to knock them down one at a time.”

 ?? CAITLIN FAW/BALTIMORE SUN ?? Johns Hopkins doctoral candidate Jinjing “Jenny” Wang says her research supports the notion that “we can change children’s math performanc­e by working with their intuitive number sense — a capacity each of us is born with, not something we have to...
CAITLIN FAW/BALTIMORE SUN Johns Hopkins doctoral candidate Jinjing “Jenny” Wang says her research supports the notion that “we can change children’s math performanc­e by working with their intuitive number sense — a capacity each of us is born with, not something we have to...

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States