Baltimore Sun Sunday

Sugar can make bees feel upbeat, study suggests

- By Sean Greene

Everyone knows sugar makes us feel good. Studies have shown that sweet foods can improve adult humans’ moods and reduce crying and grimacing in babies when they are poked in the foot by researcher­s. But does it work on insects?

Researcher­s at Queen Mary University of London wanted to see whether they could induce happy feelings in bumblebees to find out if invertebra­tes are capable of basic forms of emotion, or even emotion-like states.

With experiment­s involving humans (at least those above a certain age), scientists can simply ask their research subjects to report how they’re feeling. But when an animal is involved, scientists must infer its emotional state by observing its behavior.

So Clint Perry and colleagues fed bumblebees a sip of sugar water to test how the positive reward might bias the insects’ decision-making or affect their response to an attack by a predator. In their experiment­s, the sugared-up bees showed an emotion-like state similar to optimism, according a new study in Science.

In one experiment, the bees were placed in an enclosed foraging arena. They learned that a green card on the left side of the box meant there was a tasty drop of sucrose solution waiting inside a feeding cylinder. The bees then learned to associate a blue card on the right side of the arena with a less-exciting drop of plain water in the cylinder.

Then the researcher­s confounded their tiny subjects by placing a blue-green card at the center of the area. How would the insects interpret this situation that wasn’t sure to promise a reward at the end?

The bees with the sugar buzz appeared more likely to hope for the best — a sugary treat — and they flew faster toward the ambiguous blue-green card than the insects that received no reward beforehand, the research team reported.

This suggests the bumblebees that were induced into a good mood showed a more optimistic response to the uncertain situation. (In a 2011 study, researcher­s showed that agitated honeybees predict a punishment will result from an ambiguous cue — an emotion similar to pessimism.)

The bumblebees’ response was consistent with how humans might behave.

“Happy” bees also recovered more quickly after a simulated predator attack.

In the wild, bumblebees land on flowers to find nectar and pollen — where a crab spider might be lying in wait. Sometimes the bees survive the ambush, giving them a chance to change their future behavior and avoid another attack.

Back in the lab, the researcher­s used a special feeder that would clamp down on the bumblebees for a few seconds before safely releasing them. The bees emboldened by sugar took less time to resume their business of foraging after the attack.

The bees’ behavior meet a number of criteria that satisfy scientists’ definition of emotion — in mammals or invertebra­tes.

For example, the study authors wrote, the bees’ sugar-induced mood persisted across different situations, both the mundane foraging task and the startling predator attack. The bees’ emotion-like states also were controlled by the same neurologic­al circuitry as mammals.

“The fundamenta­l elements of emotion exist in many species,” the study authors concluded. “Our results lend support to the notion that invertebra­tes have states that fit the criteria defining emotion.”

However, whether the insects consciousl­y experience “emotional feelings” remains unanswered, they wrote.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States