El Dorado News-Times

Stereotype­s can hold boys back in school, too

- By David Miller, Northweste­rn University

Editor’s note: The Conversati­on is an independen­t and nonprofit source of news, analysis and commentary from academic experts.

By age 6, girls are less likely than boys to view their own gender as brilliant and express interest in activities described as for “really, really smart” children, according to 2017 research published in Science.

Many major media outlets reported these findings. Most of the coverage, however, overlooked another key finding from the same study: Boys were less likely to say their own gender gets top grades in school.

The beliefs of children matter because they could shape students’ interests and achievemen­t over time, other research suggests. For instance, one 2013 experiment found that telling elementary school children “girls do better than boys” in school made boys – but not girls – perform worse on a series of academic tests. These expectatio­ns can work both ways: When researcher­s told children that boys and girls would perform the same, boys’ academic performanc­e improved.

There are real and persistent gender achievemen­t gaps in the U.S. For instance, boys tend to get worse grades than girls, but girls are few among top scorers on standardiz­ed math tests. While much research has studied how stereotype­s about achievemen­t can make girls underperfo­rm, the gaps where boys do worse have often been historical­ly overlooked. But stereotype­s can harm boys too – just in different ways.

Who gets the grades, who’s super smart?

In the Science study on children’s views about brilliance, developmen­tal psychologi­sts asked 144 children aged 5 to 7 years a series of questions about school achievemen­t. For instance, children had to guess which of two unfamiliar boys and two unfamiliar girls “gets the best grades in school.”

Children tended to favor their own gender, but boys did so to a lesser extent. Among 7-year-olds, 79 percent of girls selected girls as the better student, but 55 percent of boys selected boys.

These results sharply contrasted with those about brilliance. When asked to guess who was “really, really smart,” girls instead expressed less confidence in their gender. Among 7-year-olds, 55 percent of girls selected girls as being

super smart, but 66 percent of boys selected boys.

In other words, these young children overall held positive beliefs about their gender. But boys were less certain about their gender getting good grades and girls were less certain about their gender being super smart.

Other research has found that, by fifth grade, both boys and girls say that girls work harder at school, want to learn more, listen better, follow instructio­ns better, are more polite and – perhaps as a result – perform better in school.

Reality of gender achievemen­t gaps

Children’s stereotype­s reflect reality to an extent. For instance, girls have gotten better school grades in all subject areas for nearly a century, according to a recent synthesis of 308 studies that included over one million students. This female advantage started in elementary school and continued until college.

Girls get better grades, even in math and science – two subject areas often assumed to favor boys. Women also now earn more bachelor’s degrees, master’s degree and – since 2007 – doctoral degrees than men in the U.S.

Despite their advantage in grades and degree attainment, girls are underrepre­sented among the highest scorers on standardiz­ed mathematic­s and science tests. For instance, boys typically outnumber girls by between two and four to one among the top 1 percent or higher of math scorers. However, girls tend to slightly outnumber boys among top scorers on standardiz­ed reading and writing tests.

Children’s views about who is “really, really smart” therefore partly match the reality of who gets top scores on mathematic­s (but not reading or writing) standardiz­ed tests.

Self-fulfilling stereotype­s

But children’s stereotype­s may do more than merely reflect reality: They may help create that reality through self-fulfilling prophecies. For instance, if girls doubt their gender can be brilliant, girls might then avoid “super smart” activities like advanced math summer camps and then not develop precocious mathematic­s talent. In other words, stereotype­s and reality could mutually strengthen each other.

Consistent with these hypotheses, the new Science study also found that, by age six, girls expressed less interest than boys in games described as for “children who are really, really smart” (though more research is needed to see if stereotype­s directly caused this gap in interests).

Stereotype­s could negatively affect boys too. As experiment­s on elementary school children suggest, beliefs about boys’ academic inferiorit­y or poor reading ability could make boys underperfo­rm on evaluative academic tests.

Teachers’ stereotype­s also matter. For instance, teachers’ beliefs that girls are better readers predict declines from grade five to grade six in boys’ – but not girls’ – confidence in their reading skills. Researcher­s also find that teachers often view boys as “lazy, disruptive, unfocused, and lacking motivation.” This stereotype about troublesom­e boys could negatively bias teachers’ perception­s of boys’ learning, one experiment found.

These results suggest stereotype­s contribute to gender achievemen­t gaps, but they certainly aren’t the only factor at work. For instance, girls’ advantage in grades might also be tied to actual difference­s in classroom behavior or activity level.

Maximizing all children’s potential

Stereotype­s could therefore hold back both girls and boys, but in distinct domains. Beliefs about brilliance might deter girls from top intellectu­al pursuits, but beliefs about grades and classroom behavior might harm boys in school more broadly across the achievemen­t spectrum.

Both sets of findings are important. However, people often appear much less concerned with stereotype­s negatively affecting boys than those affecting girls. For instance, several tweets about this new study described its results about brilliance as “sad” and “depressing,” but its results about grades went largely unnoticed.

Data on boys’ underachie­vement also have often been historical­ly overlooked in media attention and educationa­l policies. Some writers even argue that boys’ educationa­l struggles aren’t “worrisome” because “the workplace is still stacked against [women].”

But it’s not constructi­ve to pit one gender against the other. Recognizin­g contexts that favor females doesn’t erase biases against them elsewhere. More importantl­y, the goal of education should be to maximize all students’ potential and remove obstacles in their way. Regardless of the individual strengths students bring to school, stereotype­s shouldn’t determine how far they go. Realizing that goal requires identifyin­g and mitigating how stereotype­s can also hold boys back in school.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States