Houston Chronicle

Why the Internet isn’t making us smarter — and how to fight back

- By David Dunning David Dunning is a professor of psycholog y at the University of Michigan. This article was originally published on The Conversati­on.

In the hours since I first sat down to write this piece, my laptop tells me the National Basketball Associatio­n has had to deny that it threatened to cancel its 2017 AllStar Game over a new anti-LGBT law in North Carolina — a story repeated by many news sources including the Associated Press. The authentici­ty of that viral video of a bear chasing a female snowboarde­r in Japan has been called into question. And, no, Ted Cruz is not married to his third cousin. It’s just one among an onslaught of half-truths and even pants-on-fire lies coming as we rev up for the 2016 American election season.

The longer I study human psychology, the more impressed I am with the rich tapestry of knowledge each of us owns. We each have a brainy weave of facts, figures, rules and stories that allows us to address an astonishin­g range of everyday challenges. Contempora­ry research celebrates just how vast, organized, interconne­cted and durable that knowledge base is.

That’s the good news. The bad news is that our brains overdo it. Not only do they store helpful and essential informatio­n, they are also receptive to false belief and misinforma­tion.

Just in biology alone, many people believe that spinach is a good source of iron (sorry, Popeye), that we use less than 10 percent of our brains (no, it’s too energy-guzzling to allow that), and that some people suffer hypersensi­tivity to electromag­netic radiation (for which there is no scientific evidence).

But here’s the more concerning news. Our access to informatio­n, both good and bad, has only increased. We now have access to an Internet containing a vast store of informatio­n much bigger than any individual brain can carry — and that’s not always a good thing.

This access to the Internet’s far reaches should permit us to be smarter and better informed. People certainly assume it. A recent Yale study showed that Internet access causes people to hold inflated, illusory impres- sions of just how smart and wellinform­ed they are.

But there’s a twofold problem with the Internet that compromise­s its limitless promise.

First, just like our brains, it is receptive to misinforma­tion. In fact, the World Economic Forum lists “massive digital misinforma­tion” as a main threat to society. A survey of 50 “weight loss” websites found that only three provided sound diet advice. Another of roughly 150 YouTube videos about vaccinatio­n found that only half explicitly supported the procedure.

Rumor-mongers, politician­s, vested interests, a sensationa­lizing media and people with intellectu­al axes to grind all inject false informatio­n into the Internet.

So do a lot of well-intentione­d but misinforme­d people. In fact, a study published in the January 2016 Proceeding­s of National Academy of Science documented just how quickly dubious conspiracy theories spread across the Internet. Specifical­ly, the researcher­s compared how quickly these rumors spread across Facebook relative to stories on scientific discoverie­s. Both conspiracy theories and scientific news spread quickly, with the majority of diffusion via Facebook for both types of stories happening within a day.

Making matters worse, misinforma­tion is hard to distinguis­h from accurate fact. It often has the exact look and feel as the truth. In a series of studies Elanor Williams, Justin Kruger and I published in the Journal of Personalit­y and Social Psychology in 2013, we asked students to solve problems in intuitive physics, logic and finance. Those who consistent­ly relied on false facts or principles — and thus gave the exact same wrong answer to every problem— expressed just as much confidence in their conclusion­s as those who answered every single problem right.

So, how do we separate Internet truth from the false?

First, don’t assume misinforma­tion is obviously distinguis­hable from true informatio­n. Be careful. If the matter is impor- tant, perhaps you can start your search with the Internet; just don’t end there. Consult and consider other sources of authority. There is a reason why your doctor suffered medical school, why your financial advisor studied to gain that license.

Second, don’t do what conspiracy theorists did in the Facebook study. They readily spread stories that already fit their worldview. As such, they practiced confirmati­on bias, giving credence to evidence supporting what they already believed. As a consequenc­e, the conspiracy theories they endorsed burrowed themselves into like-minded Facebook communitie­s who rarely questioned their authentici­ty. Instead, be a skeptic. Psychologi­cal research shows that groups designatin­g one or two of its members to play devil’s advocates— questionin­g whatever conclusion the group is leaning toward — make for betterreas­oned decisions of greater quality.

If no one else is around, it pays to be your own devil’s advocate. Don’t just believe what the Internet has to say; question it. Practice a disconfirm­ation bias. If you’re looking up medical informatio­n about a health problem, don’t stop at the first diagnosis that looks right. Search for alternativ­e possibilit­ies.

In addition, look for ways in which that diagnosis might be wrong. Research shows that “considerin­g the opposite” — actively asking how a conclusion might be wrong — is a valuable exercise for reducing unwarrante­d faith in a conclusion.

Misinforma­tion and true informatio­n often look awfully alike. The key to an informed life may not require gathering informatio­n as much as it does challengin­g the ideas you already have or have recently encountere­d. This may be an unpleasant task, and an unending one, but it is the best way to ensure that your brainy intellectu­al tapestry sports only true colors.

Misinforma­tion widely abounds online, allowing users to fall victim to confirmati­on bias unless they practice vigilance and care

 ?? Getty Images ??
Getty Images

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States