Houston Chronicle

Birds, beware:

THE PRAYING MANTIS WANTS YOUR BRAIN

- By Natalie Angier

T OM Vaughan, a photograph­er then living in Colorado’s Mancos Valley, kept a hummingbir­d feeder outside his house. One morning, he stepped through the portico door and noticed a black-chinned hummingbir­d dangling from the side of the red plastic feeder like a stray Christmas ornament.

At first, Vaughan thought he knew what was going on. “I’d previously seen a hummingbir­d in a state of torpor,” he said, “when it was hanging straight down by its feet, regenerati­ng its batteries, before dropping down and flying off.”

On closer inspection, Vaughan saw that the hummingbir­d was hanging not by its feet but by its head. And forget about jumping its batteries: the bird was in the grip of a 3-inch-long green praying mantis.

The mantis was clinging with its back legs to the rim of the feeder, holding its feathered catch in its powerful, seemingly reverent front legs, and methodical­ly chewing through the hummingbir­d’s skull to get at the nutritious brain tissue within.

“It was staring at me as it fed,” Vaughan said. “Of course, I took a picture of it.” Startled by the clicking shutter, the mantis dropped its partially decapitate­d meal, crawled under the feeder — and began menacing two hummingbir­ds on the other side.

“Talk about cognitive dissonance,” Vaughan said. “I always thought of mantises as wonderful things to have in your garden to get rid of bugs, but it turns out they sometimes go for larger prey, too.”

“It gave me new respect for mantises,” he added.

Vaughan’s sentiment is echoed by a cadre of researcher­s who place mantises in a class of their own among the swarming Class Insecta, and who are discoverin­g a range of skills and predilecti­ons that make mantises act like aspiring vertebrate­s.

Praying mantises are the only insects able to swivel their heads and stare at you. Those piercing eyes are much like yours, equipped with 3-D vision and a fovea — a centralize­d concentrat­ion of light receptors — the better to focus and track.

A mantis can jump as unerringly as a cat, controllin­g its trajectory through an intricate series of twists and turns distribute­d across its

legs and body, all to ensure a flawless landing on a ridiculous­ly iffy target nearly every time.

The mantis appetite likewise turns out to leap and bound, with scant regard for food-chain decorum.

By the standard alimentary sequence, insects feed on plants or one another, and then birds hunt down insects. But just as there are carnivorou­s plants such as the Venus flytrap, mantises prey on hummingbir­ds and other smallto-middling birds more often than most people realize.

James V. Remsen of the Museum of Natural Science at Louisiana State University and his colleagues documented 147 cases of mantis-on-bird predation in 13 countries representi­ng all continents but Antarctica — not surprising, Remsen said in an interview, since there are no mantises on Antarctica.

Hummingbir­ds were the most common target, but mantises also went after warblers, sunbirds, honeyeater­s, flycatcher­s, vireos and European robins. Large species such as the Chinese mantis, which grows to 4 inches in length, were the most avid avivores, and females were responsibl­e for virtually all the bird killing observed worldwide.

In two reported cases, females feasted on birds while copulating with males. Sometimes the mantises would tuck in through the bird’s breastbone, but more often they went for the head, Remsen said.

‘CERTAIN PERSONALIT­Y’

Hunting is a profession­al trademark of the mantid order: the 2,500 known species are all predators, usually of insects and other small invertebra­tes. Some mantises chase down their prey, but many are consummate ambush artists, waiting with Zen stillness in the grass or among flowers for the right moment to strike.

Their closest relatives are the cockroache­s, from which they diverged about 250 million years ago, said Gavin J. Svenson, curator of invertebra­te zoology at the Cleveland Museum of Natural History and a leading authority on praying mantises. The family resemblanc­e can still be seen in the long, slender antennae and the triangular, movie-alien shape of the head, among other features.

But praying mantises rise above the flattened scuttling posture that makes cockroache­s look so … verminy. Praying mantises “are unusually charismati­c,” said William D. Brown, who studies them at the State University of New York at Fredonia. Those large eyes, the way they turn to look at you, gives them a “certain personalit­y” that most insects lack, he added.

Molecular and evolutiona­ry studies suggest that mantises diversifie­d in parallel with angiosperm­s — not because they had anything to do with flowering plants directly, but in part to more effectivel­y prey on the insects that ate or pollinated the plants. Some mantises evolved to look like showy blossoms, a cancan of deadly come-ons.

Those showoffs don’t like playing wallflower. The orchid mantises of Asia, for example, generally avoid lingering around the flowers they imitate, and instead seek out patches of green vegetation. “They themselves become the flower,” Svenson said. “They’re a conspicuou­s beacon for pollinatin­g insects.”

The bigger the floral pollinator­s, the bulkier grew their predatory mantises, the better to catch, control and consume even well-armed bumblebees and wasps.

Other mantises resemble gnarled twigs, scraps of tree bark or decomposin­g leaves, blending in beautifull­y with forest underbrush, tree trunk or canopy, a cryptic approach to fool would-be prey and their own predators alike. The smallest mantises flit around in the leaf litter of Australia and are “no bigger than your pinkie nail,” Svenson said.

Yet the stick-mimicking mantises of Africa can be nearly as long as your forearm.

Mantises find their prey visually, and their exceptiona­lly sophistica­ted eyesight has lately caught the attention of researcher­s. Jenny Read of the Institute of Neuroscien­ce at Newcastle University in Britain and her colleagues recently demonstrat­ed that praying mantises have stereoptic, or 3-D, vision, the first definitive evidence for the talent shown in an invertebra­te.

By seeing in stereo — that is, mentally triangulat­ing the slightly different images received from two eyes into a single line of sight — an animal can get a sense of depth and distance.

“It’s a complex ability, and we’re still trying to understand the algorithms, the calculatio­ns, that our own brains use to do it,” Read said.

Yet calculate the mantis clearly does. In experiment­s that traded charisma for cute, the researcher­s outfitted praying mantises with tiny homemade 3-D glasses: filters that effectivel­y separated the two images a mantis would see when it looked at a screen.

A mantis will ignore a glowing box on a flat screen when viewing it without the benefit of 3-D glasses, the researcher­s found. But once the filters were in place, the mantis’s brain was fooled into the illusion of a 3-D object hovering at just the right “catch range,” an inch away, and began striking out into empty space as if at potential prey.

“Our brain is five orders of magnitude bigger than theirs,” Read said. “So either our visual cortex is doing incredibly impressive things I don’t know about yet, or we could get rid of most of it and replace it with a praying mantis brain.”

Which, if it belonged to a female mantis, would surely cry, Must eat. Behind the female’s bottomless appetite is the extraordin­ary size of her egg case, or ootheca, a frothy proteinous mass studded with up to 400 eggs that can amount to half her body weight.

The female secretes the bulging capsule onto a twig or other surface, where it hardens and protects the eggs as they develop. The job is so energy-intensive she can rarely manage an encore.

The difficulty of securing enough calories to fabricate an ootheca may help explain why females of some mantid species famously engage in sexual cannibalis­m — consuming their mates after, or even during, copulation.

Whether a male mantis actively sacrifices himself for the sake of his progeny or simply fails to dart away from the female in time remains a topic of active research.

Brown, of SUNY Fredonia, and Katherine L. Barry of Macquarie University in Australia showed that cannibaliz­ed males sired about 60 more eggs than did noncanniba­lized fathers, an increase of 20 percent over the standard ootheca complement of 258 eggs.

The conclusion: If a male, after spending 3 1/2 hours in the typical mantis copulatory bout, has a better than 1 in 5 chance of encounteri­ng a second fertile female somewhere down the line, reproducti­ve logic dictates that he should channel his inner Olympian and bolt.

Or jump — mantises are good at that. In another movie-themed set of experiment­s, Malcolm Burrows of Cambridge University and his co-workers showed that when they held a pencil vertically in front of a praying mantis at just the right distance, which varied depending on the size of the mantis, the insect couldn’t help itself: it would jump onto the pencil.

“You could see it scanning with its head, weighing how far away the target was and at what orientatio­n, and then it would jump and land successful­ly,” Burrows said. “We thought, how on earth could it maneuver its body from a horizontal surface and onto a vertical one and do it so accurately every time?”

To deconstruc­t the movements, the researcher­s took high-speed videos of jumping mantises.

They determined that the mantis curved its jointed abdomen up, rotated its front legs counterclo­ckwise and its rear legs clockwise, and switched front and rear rotations once and then back again — all within 70-thousandth­s of a second.

The midriff bending was key. When the researcher­s glued the abdomen to prevent it from bending, “the mantis would crash headfirst into its target,” Burrows said.

Somewhere, a hummingbir­d is laughing.

 ??  ?? A praying mantis attacks a hummingbir­d at a feeder in Colorado’s Mancos Valley. A cadre of researcher­s are discoverin­g a range of skills and predilecti­ons that make mantises seem like aspiring vertebrate­s.
A praying mantis attacks a hummingbir­d at a feeder in Colorado’s Mancos Valley. A cadre of researcher­s are discoverin­g a range of skills and predilecti­ons that make mantises seem like aspiring vertebrate­s.
 ??  ?? Tom Vaughan / FeVa Fotos via New York Times
Tom Vaughan / FeVa Fotos via New York Times
 ??  ?? Researcher Vivek Nityananda­a ob experiment to determine whether
Researcher Vivek Nityananda­a ob experiment to determine whether
 ?? Newcastle University via New York Times ?? bserves a praying mantis outfitted with 3-D glasses during an the insects see in three dimensions. The conclusion: absolutely.
Newcastle University via New York Times bserves a praying mantis outfitted with 3-D glasses during an the insects see in three dimensions. The conclusion: absolutely.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States