Imperial Valley Press

Eyewitness identifica­tion is convincing, is it reliable?

- MATTHEW MANGINO Matthew T. Mangino is of counsel with Luxenberg, Garbett, Kelly & George P.C. His book, “The Executione­r’s Toll, 2010,” was released by McFarland Publishing. You can reach him at www.mattmangin­o.com and follow him on Twitter @MatthewTMa­ng

For decades eyewitness identifica­tion was considered the gold standard when it came to evidence used to gain a conviction.

In the famous courtroom drama “12 Angry Men,” rated by the American Bar Associatio­n as one of the 25 greatest legal movies of all time, juror No. 8, played by Henry Fonda, earnestly advocated for a not-guilty verdict.

Fonda started out as the only notguilty vote. The turning point of the deliberati­ons occurred when an older juror recalled that the state’s prized eyewitness, who had observed the murder through her window as she laid in bed, had red marks on her nose left from wearing eyeglasses. The older juror asked a reserved bespectacl­ed juror, “Do you wear your glasses when you go to bed?” The bespectacl­ed juror responded, “No, I don’t. No one wears eyeglasses to bed.”

Stephen Handelman, editor-in-chief of The Crime Report, an online news service, wrote recently, eyewitness identifica­tion is “often the most dramatic moment in a TV crime procedural: An eyewitness is brought into the station house to identify a crime suspect from a lineup. Tension builds as detectives await the definitive truth that will lead to a conviction.”

Eyewitness identifica­tion can be convincing, but is it reliable?

More than 75,000 prosecutio­ns every year are based entirely on eyewitness identifica­tion. Some of those identifica­tions are erroneous. Advances in the social sciences and technology have cast a new light on eyewitness identifica­tion.

Hundreds of studies on eyewitness identifica­tion have been published in profession­al and academic journals.

One study by University of Virginia Law School professor Brandon L. Garrett, found that eyewitness misidentif­ications contribute­d to wrongful conviction­s in 76 percent of the cases overturned by DNA evidence.

U.S. Supreme Court Justice Sonia Sotomayor has acknowledg­ed the shortcomin­gs of eyewitness testimony. She wrote, “eyewitness identifica­tions’ unique confluence of features -- their unreliabil­ity, susceptibi­lity to suggestion, powerful impact on the jury, and resistance to the ordinary tests of the adversaria­l process -- can undermine the fairness of a trial.”

What can cause an eyewitness to misidentif­y a suspect? There are a number of factors: Poor lighting, the crime occurred quickly, the presence of a gun, and the fact that the perpetrato­r is a different race than the witness. The police can, as well, intentiona­lly or unintentio­nally influence an eyewitness’ identifica­tion.

A number of states have put in place a process to review and revise state rules for how judges and jurors treat evidence from police lineups and photo arrays. States are utilizing practices supported by years of research.

A recent research paper published in the Journal of Research in Memory and Cognition argues it’s long overdue to replace the current “antiquated” approach to eyewitness identifica­tion as it relates to lineups and photo arrays.

The authors, Neil Brewer of Founders University in Australia and James Doyle, a Boston-based criminal defense attorney propose a “screening” method in which eyewitness­es are asked to grade the probabilit­y that one of an array of persons presented to them in a lineup matches the perpetrato­r of a crime.

According to Handelman, the alternativ­e approach replaces the “categorica­l traditiona­l identifica­tion decision with a procedure whereby the witness simply rates how confident they are that each person in the lineup is the culprit.”

“The confidence ratings ... provide a probabilis­tic guide as to whether the culprit is in the lineup,” wrote Handelman quoting the report. “And this, of course is the fundamenta­l question being asked.”

Former U.S. Supreme Court Justice William J. Brennan Jr. wrote in a dissenting opinion nearly 35 years ago, “There is almost nothing more convincing than a live human being who takes the stand, points a finger at the defendant, and says, ‘That’s the one!’”

No one can challenge that impact of eyewitness identifica­tion. However, it is clear from the research and the growing number of exoneratio­ns that the reliabilit­y of eyewitness identifica­tion falls far below its impact. Without meaningful reform the threat of convicting the innocent continues.

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States