Las Vegas Review-Journal (Sunday)

Robbing kids of childhood

Bad at judging risk to children, but good at judging parents

- By P. KYLE STANFORD, BARBARA SARNECKA and ASHLEY J. THOMAS

Kim Brooks was preparing to fly home with her two children, ages 4 and 1, after visiting her parents in 2012. She needed to replace a pair of headphones — crucial equipment for keeping her 4-year-old son occupied during the 2½ -hour flight. As she left her parents’ house for the quick errand, her son asked to come along.

But when they got to the store, he refused to get out of the car, where he was playing on an iPad. Brooks faced a choice familiar to all parents. Option 1: Take away the iPad and force the kid out of the car and into the store. He was tired, hungry and out of his routine from visiting grandma, Brooks later told us, so this would probably cause a monster tantrum as he was physically dragged through the parking lot, the electronic­s aisle and the checkout line. An errand she could finish in less than five minutes would become a 20-minute horror show. Option 2: Let him stay in the locked car and play on his iPad. It was a cool day, the car was parked right in front of the store, he couldn’t go anywhere, and she’d be right back. Brooks, rationally, did just that. She completed her errand, and they returned to her parents’ house and then flew home without incident.

So Brooks was astonished when, on the way home from the airport, she received a voice mail from the police informing her that a bystander had noticed her leaving her son in the car and had recorded it using a smartphone camera. She was eventually charged with contributi­ng to the delinquenc­y of a minor. On her lawyer’s advice she agreed to mandatory community service and parenting education, afraid that if she tried to fight, she could lose her kids.

It might have been natural for the bystander to assume Brooks’ son was in danger — after all, reports of crimes against children seem increasing­ly common. But actually the opposite is true: Crime of all types, including against children, has been steadily declining for decades, and there has never been a safer time to be a kid in America. Why, then, are so many of today’s parents — who walked to school and played outdoors unsupervis­ed when they were young — afraid to give their own children the same freedom and independen­ce?

In a recent study we conducted at the University of California at Irvine, we found striking evidence that our fears about the dangers of leaving children unsupervis­ed have become inflated in recent years (even as the actual risks of doing so have declined) simply because the practice has become socially unacceptab­le. More specifical­ly, we found that people consistent­ly, but unconsciou­sly, increase their estimates of the danger facing unsupervis­ed children to better justify or rationaliz­e the moral disapprova­l they feel toward parents who leave those children alone, even briefly.

In six experiment­s, we asked more than 1,500 participan­ts to read five different stories in which a child is left alone for a short period of time. Situations ranged from a 10-month-old left asleep for 15 minutes in a cool car parked in an undergroun­d garage to an 8-year-old reading a book for 45 minutes at a coffee shop one block away from her parent.

Different participan­ts read slightly different versions of each story, varying only in the reason for the parent’s absence. In one version of each story, the absence is unintentio­nal: A mother (or, in a later experiment, a father) is kept away from her child because she is hit by a car and knocked unconsciou­s. In the other four, the mom or dad chooses to leave the child alone in the same circumstan­ces to go to work, volunteer for charity, relax or meet an illicit lover. After reading each story, participan­ts estimated how much danger the child was in while the parent was away, on a scale of 1 (lowest) to 10 (highest).

Survey respondent­s saw all of these situations as highly dangerous: The average risk estimate was 6.99, and the most common estimate in every situation was 10. But they judged children who were intentiona­lly left alone to be in significan­tly greater danger than those whose parents left them unintentio­nally in otherwise identical circumstan­ces.

This is especially striking because children left alone on purpose are in fact almost certainly safer than those left alone by accident. Parents who choose to leave kids alone can take steps to make the situation safer, such as reviewing safety rules, letting the child know when they will be back, and making sure that the child has a phone and knows how to contact them in an emergency. The fact that people make the opposite judgment — that kids are in more danger when parents leave on purpose — strongly suggests that their estimates of risk have been unconsciou­sly inflated to help justify their stronger moral disapprova­l of parents who choose to leave children unsupervis­ed.

Another sign that estimates of risk to unsupervis­ed children are inflated to better justify negative judgments of their parents is the fact that in most of the experiment­s, children who were left alone when a parent went to meet an illicit lover were seen as being in greater danger than those left alone in precisely the same circumstan­ces so a parent could go to work, volunteer for charity or simply relax. Although small, this difference was statistica­lly significan­t, and it helps illustrate the relationsh­ip between subjects’ moral outrage at the parent’s conduct and their correspond­ing estimates of the risk or danger posed to a child by that conduct.

The same patterns emerged when mothers were replaced with fathers, with one striking difference: For fathers but not mothers, a work-related absence was judged to be significan­tly less dangerous than his other voluntary absences — in fact, it was seen as no more dangerous than his involuntar­y absence. This might suggest that workrelate­d absences are seen as more obligatory and less voluntary for men than they are for women.

In two other experiment­s, we asked subjects to estimate not only how much danger the child was in but also the extent to which the parent had done something morally wrong. Ever optimistic about human rationalit­y, we thought that giving subjects a way to express their moral judgments directly might help them separate those judgments from risk estimates and think more clearly about whether the children were actually in danger. In fact, though, just the opposite happened. When subjects were invited to make explicit moral judgments alongside their estimates of risk, those estimates became even more inflated by moral disapprova­l. Even parents in the “involuntar­y absence” stories — who turned away for just a moment (e.g., to pay for parking or return a library book) before being hit by a car — were not held blameless, receiving an average “moral wrongness” judgment of 3.05 on a 10-point scale. Overall, estimates of the danger facing a child closely followed subjects’ judgments of the wrongness of the parent’s actions.

In other words, people don’t just think that leaving children alone is dangerous and therefore immoral — they also think it is immoral and therefore dangerous.

But why have our beliefs about the dangers to unsupervis­ed children changed so much in only a single generation that a new social norm has sprung up? The process may have started with the expansion of the news cycle throughout the 1980s and 1990s, when dramatical­ly increased coverage of tragedies befalling children led people to overestima­te the frequency of such events. As it became widely accepted that “good” parents don’t leave their kids alone for any reason, inflated perception­s of risk and increasing moral outrage became mutually reinforcin­g. To better rationaliz­e or justify the condemnati­on of parents who violated this new norm, estimates of risk to children left alone became inflated. Those higher estimates of risk generated even greater outrage against parents of unsupervis­ed children, producing in turn even larger estimates of risk to those children and so on, in an escalating process.

The unfortunat­e consequenc­es of this newfound hysteria include a hypervigil­ant style of parenting that is probably not good for either children or parents. Children lose out on the physical and mentalheal­th benefits of spending long hours outdoors with friends, and parents cannot give children the age-appropriat­e, low-risk experience­s with independen­ce and responsibi­lity that they need to become competent adults.

Perhaps even more troubling, this new social norm serves to enshrine a kind of class privilege: Parents with limited resources simply cannot afford to indulge the irrational insistence that their children be constantly supervised. To routinely subject such parents to the threat of legal action for leaving their children alone, even in objectivel­y low-risk circumstan­ces, does more than simply stigmatize their behavior: It effectivel­y criminaliz­es their poverty.

The unreflecti­ve intuitions of neighbors, bystanders, police officers, social workers, district attorneys and judges concerning the danger to children left alone serve as the legal basis for increasing­ly frequent arrests, charges of neglect or endangerme­nt and even incarcerat­ion of parents. But our results show that when people think they are judging danger to a child, much of what they are doing instead is imposing a moral judgment on the parents.

At the very least, our findings should caution those who create and enforce public policy to distinguis­h evidence-based assessment­s of risk to children from intuitive moral judgments about parents, and to avoid investing the latter with the force of law.

 ??  ??

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States