Las Vegas Review-Journal (Sunday)

Memory frailty may play into Kavanaugh issue

Nominee, accuser both believe their ‘truth’

- By Malcolm Ritter

NEW YORK — She says he sexually assaulted her; he denies it. Is somebody deliberate­ly lying? Not necessaril­y.

Experts say that because of how memory works, it’s possible that both Supreme Court nominee Brett Kavanaugh and Christine Blasey Ford — the woman who says a drunken Kavanaugh pinned her to a bed and groped her at a party when they were teenagers in the early 1980s — believe what they say.

And which one of them believes his or her version more strongly is no tipoff to what really happened.

“Confidence is not a good guide to whether or not someone is telling the truth,” said Nora Newcombe, a psychology professor at Temple University in Philadelph­ia. “If they think they’re telling the truth, they could plausibly both be confident about it.”

As the nation ponders the accusation­s from Ford that could derail Kavanaugh’s nomination, the possibilit­y that one of them simply got it wrong has been floated on Capitol Hill.

Experts say a person’s memory is not like a video recorder, perfectly capturing an objective record of everything that happens for later retrieval.

“Memory is mostly true but sometimes unreliable,” said psychologi­st Jennifer Talarico of Lafayette College in Easton, Pennsylvan­ia. Generally, “we get the gist mostly right.”

Your beliefs and expectatio­ns shape what you perceive in your life and how you later remember those events, researcher­s say.

“You are constructi­ng the reality out there as it happens, and therefore you get stuck with that … as the most accurate you can have for your memory,” said David Rubin, a professor of psychology and neuroscien­ce at Duke University. “That’s all you have to base your memory on.”

So in a situation where a woman fears being raped by a man, her memories might be shaped by that fear into a recollecti­on that overestima­tes the threat, whereas the man might consider it “just playing around” and simply forget it later on, Rubin said. And both could be completely honest about their recollecti­ons.

Rubin noted the obvious fact that people can forget things they did while drunk. But he said the man in his scenario could forget about the event even if he had been sober.

Typically, when people make mistakes in recalling an event, they unknowingl­y slip in details that would be typical on such occasions, Talarico said.

When it comes to emotionall­y charged events, it’s typical to remember a central person or an item such as a gun but forget the context and details, Newcombe said. “You have this vivid central thing and everything else is fuzzy,” she said. “Emotion makes one thing go up and the other go down.”

So it would be wrong to challenge Ford’s memory of the alleged incident over inability to recall details, Newcombe said.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States