Los Angeles Times

Black men’s plight as ‘superhuman’

Bias that minority males are bigger, more menacing than white men may play role in police shootings.

- AMINA KHAN amina.khan@latimes.com Twitter: @aminawrite

People consistent­ly perceive black men to be bigger and more muscular than they actually are — and as more of a threat — than they do white men of the same size, a new study shows.

The findings, presented in the Journal of Personalit­y and Social Psychology, shed light on the deadly link that such misconcept­ions might have to police shootings of unarmed black men.

“Across a range of different stimuli and dependent variables, perceivers showed a consistent and strong bias to perceive young Black men as larger and more capable of harm than young White men (at least among non-Black participan­ts),” the study authors wrote. “Such perception­s may have disturbing consequenc­es for how both civilians and law enforcemen­t personnel perceive and behave toward Black individual­s.”

Black men and women are physically stereotype­d in many contexts — described as “natural” athletes instead of “intelligen­t” ones, for example, or able to bear inordinate amounts of pain and suffering in depictions in entertainm­ent and pop culture.

“Black people are often dehumanize­d but also super-humanized; this means that people will attribute superhuman characteri­stics to black people,” said lead author John Paul Wilson, a social psychologi­st at Montclair State University in New Jersey. “So we actually wondered if this stereotype might be powerful enough that it would even show up in a pretty basic way — in concrete perception­s of others’ physical characteri­stics.”

In a series of experiment­s, the researcher­s tested whether study participan­ts viewed white and black men differentl­y, even when they were largely of the same height and weight. For example, when shown individual images of 45 white and 45 black faces taken from a college football recruiting website, participan­ts estimated the black football players’ heights and weights to be higher than those of the white players (even though, incidental­ly, the white players were everso-slightly taller and heavier than the black players pictured).

“I don’t think most black men or kids or parents would really be too surprised to read about these results either,” Wilson said. “I think it’s a finding that would ring true to many. But we did think that, regardless of that … it was important to do this work and I think it’s important to continue to follow up on it.”

Other rounds of the experiment showed that black men were also perceived as stronger and more muscular than equivalent white men, with more upper body strength (a proxy for fighting ability).

The scientists next asked participan­ts to imagine themselves in a fight with one of the men whose faces were pictured in the previous study. Overall, the participan­ts gauged black men to be “more capable of harm” than white men, even though the scientists had equalized both white and black men for size.

“The results strongly suggest that this race-based bias in perceived formidabil­ity results from perceivers’ beliefs about race (i.e., stereotype­s), rather than an accurate inference of physical size based on facial cues,” the authors wrote. “This serves as initial evidence that biased judgments of size may influence downstream inferences of physical capabiliti­es.”

So if people perceive black men to be more harmful, do they also think it’s more justifiabl­e to use force against them? To probe that question, a follow-up experiment asked participan­ts to imagine each of the white and black faces as belonging to a man who was unarmed but behaving aggressive­ly with a police officer, and then judge whether it was appropriat­e to use force to subdue him.

Their answers revealed that, as the scientists had suspected, participan­ts judged the use of force as more appropriat­e against a black man than against a white one.

“People judged Black men as larger and more harmful than White men, thus rendering them more suitable recipients of physical force,” the researcher­s wrote.

The scientists included black participan­ts in some experiment­s, finding that they also seemed to rate the black men in the images as larger than the white men — which means they may have also absorbed this cultural stereotype. They did not, however, expect black men to be more capable of harm, as the white and other nonblack study participan­ts did.

“Thus, although Black individual­s may have learned the same cultural stereotype­s about the size of Black men,” the scientists noted, “they do not seem to apply these mispercept­ions the same way that nonBlack people do.”

The scientists also found that men with more stereotypi­cally “Afrocentri­c” features — whether they were black or white — were also rated higher on the “formidabil­ity” scale by study participan­ts.

“Such perception­s can have dangerous consequenc­es,” the scientists wrote. “For example, participan­ts in a first-person shooter task mistakenly shot unarmed White targets who looked less prototypic­ally White and more Afrocentri­c-looking individual­s may be punished more severely in court even to the point of execution.”

When police officers shoot unarmed black men, the justificat­ions given after the fact typically involve descriptio­ns of the victim’s size and formidabil­ity — and those descriptio­ns often turn out to have been wildly inaccurate.

Among the many examples in the media in recent years, the scientists pointed to Dontre Hamilton, shot 14 times by a white police officer in Milwaukee who testified that Hamilton was “muscular” and “most definitely would have overpowere­d … me or pretty much any officer I can think of.” Hamilton, according to his autopsy, was just 5 feet, 7 inches and weighed 169 pounds.

“Unfortunat­ely, we don’t yet have much in the way of solutions,” Wilson said. “I certainly would hope that in the future we could get to a place where we are able to make recommenda­tions of how to counteract this bias.”

Wilson was quick to note that these were lab studies on laypeople; still, the findings may be able to inform how we think about the police use of force in these situations.

“I would also hope to in the future work with the police, for example,” he added. “I think it’s important that we bring our science to the public and form partnershi­ps with various types of stakeholde­rs, certainly including police department­s.”

Wilson emphasized that this race-based size and threat bias was not strongly predicted by explicit racial prejudice; that is, people who may not consider themselves racist may also be operating under these assumption­s.

“This is not just about people having some kind of racial animus,” he said. Being aware that these biases may influence perception­s is a first step to dealing with them.

“If you’re aware that people tend to show this bias, it may trigger you to think about whether the reality, the perception­s that you are experienci­ng, are objective,” he said. “Because we often know that our perception­s are not that objective.”

 ?? Carrie Antlfinger Associated Press ?? NATE HAMILTON, speaking to reporters in 2015, is the brother of Dontre Hamilton, who was fatally shot by a white Milwaukee police officer who later testified that the 169-pound man would have overpowere­d him.
Carrie Antlfinger Associated Press NATE HAMILTON, speaking to reporters in 2015, is the brother of Dontre Hamilton, who was fatally shot by a white Milwaukee police officer who later testified that the 169-pound man would have overpowere­d him.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States