Los Angeles Times

After Koko and Washoe, are animals commoditie­s?

- Eugene Linden is the author of the books “Apes, Men and Language,” “Silent Partners,” “The Parrot’s Lament” and “The Octopus and the Orangutan,” and many articles about animal intelligen­ce. By Eugene Linden

Koko the gorilla died on June 19. She and a female chimpanzee named Washoe (who died in 2007) played an outsized role in changing how we view animal intelligen­ce. Their accomplish­ments inaugurate­d deep soul-searching among us humans about the moral basis of our relationsh­ip with nature. Koko and Washoe have made it much more difficult for us to treat animals as commoditie­s, in any way we wish.

I knew the two great apes when I was young and they were young, and I”ve closely followed the scientific, philosophi­cal and moral upheavals they precipitat­ed over the last five decades. In the 1960s and ’70s, they learned to use American sign language, and they came to understand that words could be combined to convey new meanings. It threw the scientific world into a tizzy, implying that sentience and language were not ours alone, that there was a continuum in higher mental abilities that linked animals and humans.

The problem for science remains unresolved: 3,000 years into the investigat­ion of signal human attributes and we still don’t have rigorous ways to define language and intelligen­ce that are agreed on and can be empiricall­y tested. There remain a number of scientists who don’t think Koko and Washoe accomplish­ed anything at all. Even if a scientist accepts one of the definition­s of language that do exist, it’s nearly impossible to test it in animals because what is being examined is inherently subjective, and science demands objective, verifiable results.

Consider how hard it is to prove a lie beyond a reasonable doubt in court. Then consider trying to prove lying in an animal in accord with the much stricter standards of science.

As difficult as proving it may be, examples of apes lying abound. When Koko was 5, I was playing a chase game with her. When I caught her, she gave me a small bite. Penny Patterson, Koko’s lifelong foster parent and teacher, was there, and, in sign language, demanded, “What did you do?” Koko signed, “Not teeth.” Penny wasn’t buying it: “Koko, you lied.”

“Bad again Koko bad again,” Koko admitted.

“Koko, you lied.” But what was Koko’s intent — a central issue when it comes to proving a lie. What was actually going on in her head when she made the gestures for “not teeth?” As if that weren’t inscrutabl­e enough, one of the guiding principles of scientific investigat­ions of animal intelligen­ce is what’s known as Morgan’s Canon: Scientists must not impute a higher mental ability if a behavior can be explained by something more primitive, for example, simple error.

Analogousl­y, about 50 years ago, on a pond in Oklahoma, Washoe saw a swan and made the signs for “water” and “bird.” Was she simply noting a bird and water, or was she combining two of the signs she knew to describe an animal for which she had no specific word? The debate continued for decades and was unresolved when she died.

Since Washoe made those signs, there have been many more instances of apes combining words to describe something, but these examples still don’t prove they can combine words to arrive at a novel term, even if it seems obvious that they can. Faced with these ambiguitie­s, many scientists have moved to studying whether animals can accomplish specific cognitive tasks, and a welter of credible findings show sophistica­ted abilities in animals ranging from crows to elephants.

Although science struggles with questions of general intelligen­ce, language and intent, the public is in the “it’s obvious” camp, readily accepting evidence of animal sentience. The latest objects of fascinatio­n are the octopus — a relative of the clam! — and fish. Stories of cephalopod escape and problemsol­ving regularly go viral, and to the consternat­ion of sushi lovers, John Balcomb’s book, “What a Fish Knows,” provides copious evidence that fish know a lot.

We tend to see animals as either personalit­ies or commoditie­s, or sometimes both. When I wrote about octopus intelligen­ce, I was amused by one octopus-oriented website that divided its space between stories of smart octopuses and recipes for cooking them. Perhaps the most extraordin­ary example of our schizophre­nic view of animals occurred some years back when a chimp colony that included sign-language-using apes was disbanded and many of these onetime celebritie­s were shipped to a medical research lab to be used in Hepatitis B and AIDS drug testing.

I knew these chimps too, and visited them in their new environmen­t. They were desperate to communicat­e with their human captors, but the staff didn’t know sign language. So insistent were Booee and Bruno with their signing that one handler put up a poster outside the cages showing some basic signs to help the humans respond. When I was there, three days after Booee had arrived, he was signing agitatedly for food and drink. But what I think he really wanted was reassuranc­e: If the humans would respond to “gimme drink,” things were going to be OK.

Teaching Koko, Washoe and other animals some level of human and invented languages promised experiment­ers insight into the animal mind. But the animals seemed to seize on these languages as a way to make their wishes — and thoughts — known to their strange, bipedal wardens, who had no ability or interest in learning the animals’ communicat­ion system. For Koko, I believe, sign language was a way to make the best of a truly unnatural situation, and so she signed.

Science doesn’t know if great apes can invent terms or if they tell lies. And the tension between whether we view and treat animals as personalit­ies or as commoditie­s lives on. The truth is, Koko, Washoe and many other animals who have had two-way conversati­ons with the people around them shatter the moral justificat­ion for the latter.

 ?? Knight-Ridder Tribune ??
Knight-Ridder Tribune

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States