Los Angeles Times

‘Genius’ winners discuss research

Science behind how we see faces

- By Melissa Healy

We talk to MacArthur fellows Doris Tsao and Sarah Stewart, recognized for work on how the brain “sees” and planets formed.

How does the brain turn millions of electrical impulses into objects we recognize?

That question is at the heart of Doris Tsao’s research.

The Caltech visual neuroscien­tist uses brain imaging technology, electrical recording techniques and mathematic­al modeling in her search for answers. That quest got a boost Thursday as Tsao was named to the 2018 class of MacArthur fellows.

“I study how we see — how the brain sees — and this is important because it’s a way of understand­ing how the brain works,” said Tsao, who was born in China and raised in College Park, Md. “And the brain, of course, is who we are.”

Tsao, 42, has explored several aspects of visual processing, including the perception of depth and color. But her most notable line of research seeks to uncover the fundamenta­l neural principles that underlie one of the brain’s most highly specialize­d and socially important tasks: recognizin­g a face.

The so-called genius award provides fellows with $625,000 to spend over five

years, with no strings attached. Tsao spoke with The Times about the work that caught the MacArthur Foundation’s attention.

What inspired you to study the way we recognize faces?

I first became interested in vision in sixth grade, when I woke up one morning and wondered whether space is infinite or not.

This is a problem that’s been tackled by philosophe­rs, physicists and vision scientists.

The way vision scientists think about this problem is: “How does the electrical activity of neurons in the brain construct our subjective perception of space?”

From space, I became interested in how 3-D objects are perceived, and this led to my current interest in object recognitio­n.

And faces in particular?

Yes, indeed. This ability to recognize faces is remarkable.

Is this something everyone is good at?

There are certain people called prosopagno­sics who are actually extremely bad at recognizin­g faces. One guy with this condition commented that he didn’t understand why, in the movies, the robbers always cover their faces. To him, it was as incomprehe­nsible as covering the arm.

When it comes to recognizin­g faces, how do humans stack up against other primates?

The jury is still out. Even among humans, there’s a lot of variation due to experience.

How can you tell what’s going on inside the brain?

There was evidence from humans for the existence of an area dedicated to processing faces. This area was discovered using a technique called fMRI [or functional magnetic resonance imaging] that measures blood flow in the brain.

I decided to try to find a similar area in monkeys so that I could then study it with electrophy­siology, a much more sensitive technique that lets us pick up electrical signals from single neurons.

If you figure out how the brain recognize faces, what else will it tell you?

We can learn a lot about how any object is represente­d from understand­ing how the brain represents faces.

Faces are like a Rosetta stone. The brain uses a certain set of rules to convert different face images into patterns of electrical activity.

It turns out that these rules look very similar to those that govern how we recognize other objects.

How do you know?

We’ve found that, in macaque monkeys, a region called the inferior temporal cortex — part of the visual cortex — has six distinct but densely interconne­cted “patches” of cells that become highly active when those monkeys are assessing the faces of other macaques. We call those “face patches.”

Individual cells in these regions act like rulers, measuring faces along different axes of a large “face space.”

What’s that?

Face space is similar to our familiar 3-D space, but each point represents a face rather than a spatial location, and it has much more than just three dimensions — probably at least 100.

For our familiar 3-D space, any point can be described by 3 coordinate­s (x,y,z). For a 100-dimensiona­l face space, any point can be described by 100 coordinate­s. Each cell in a face patch is essentiall­y measuring one of these 100 coordinate­s. Together, they can determine the exact face being seen. How do you test something like this?

We learned, for each cell, what coordinate of face space it was in charge of measuring. Then we showed an animal a mystery face (that we weren’t allowed to peek at).

Just using the electrical responses of the face cells, we could reconstruc­t the face the animal was seeing. It turned out to match exactly the face it was actually seeing.

Do we store memories of faces using the same kinds of templates that we use to recognize them?

We are studying this process.

What’s very cool is that the brain does face recognitio­n in two separate steps. First it computes a code for every face, including unfamiliar ones. Then it compares these codes to memories.

One of the joys of studying the brain is that I get to actually peek inside the carpenter shop and see each step that the brain is using to construct our perception of reality.

There’s a difference between recognizin­g a face and being able to conjure a face from the past. What’s that about?

I wish I knew.

This interview has been edited for length and clarity.

 ?? John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation ?? DORIS TSAO of Caltech seeks to uncover the fundamenta­l neural principles that underlie one of the brain’s most highly specialize­d tasks: recognizin­g a face.
John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation DORIS TSAO of Caltech seeks to uncover the fundamenta­l neural principles that underlie one of the brain’s most highly specialize­d tasks: recognizin­g a face.
 ?? John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation ?? SARAH STEWART with students in her Shock Compressio­n Lab at UC Davis, where high-impact shooting experiment­s help explain how planets formed.
John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation SARAH STEWART with students in her Shock Compressio­n Lab at UC Davis, where high-impact shooting experiment­s help explain how planets formed.
 ?? Roman Cho John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation ?? “I STUDY how we see — how the brain sees — and this is important because it’s a way of understand­ing how the brain works,” said Doris Tsao of Caltech.
Roman Cho John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation “I STUDY how we see — how the brain sees — and this is important because it’s a way of understand­ing how the brain works,” said Doris Tsao of Caltech.

Newspapers in English

Newspapers from United States